- Orthographies and dyslexia
-
Main articles: Dyslexia and Dyslexia research
The complexity of a language's orthography, or writing system, can be a significant contributing factor to the difficulties experienced by dyslexic readers.[1] Current psycholinguistic models of dyslexia are "largely developed on the basis of alphabetic writing systems such as English",[2] but the amount of research on some logographic orthographies, Chinese in particular,[3] is also fairly significant. However, little research has been done on syllabic writing systems, and "cross-linguistic studies of the acquired dyslexias and dysgraphias are scarce." [2]
Contents
Dyslexia and orthographic features
The effects of orthographic depth on dyslexia
The complexity of a language's orthography is directly related to the difficulty of learning to read it. Orthographic complexity also contributes to how dyslexia manifests in readers of different languages.[4]
Deep orthographies are writing systems, such as those of English and Arabic, that do not have a one-to-one correspondence between sounds (phonemes) and the letters (graphemes) that represent them. Shallow orthographies have a one-to-one relationship between graphemes and phonemes, and the spelling of words is very consistent.
For languages with more shallow orthographies, such as Italian and Finnish, new readers have few problems learning to decode words. As a result, children learn to read relatively quickly.[1] Most dyslexic readers of shallow orthographies learn to decode words with relative ease, but they tend to have more difficulty with reading fluency and comprehension. The hallmark symptom of dyslexia in a shallow orthography is (a comparatively low) speed of rapid automatized naming.[5]
For languages with relatively deep orthographies, such as English and French, new readers have a great deal more difficulty learning to decode words. As a result, children learn to read more slowly.[1] Research has shown that the hallmark symptoms of dyslexia in a deep orthography are a deficit in phonological awareness and difficulty reading at the word level.[5] For these dyslexic readers, learning to decode words may take a long time—indeed, in the deepest orthographies the hallmark symptom of dyslexia is the inability to read at the word level—but many dyslexic readers have relatively fewer problems with fluency and comprehension once some level of decoding has been mastered.
Studies between German and English have shown that the greater depth of English orthography had a "marked adverse effect on reading skills" among dyslexic children.[6][7]
Dyslexia in different types of orthography
There are a number of different types of writing systems, or orthographies, and they do not necessarily depend on the same neurological skill sets.[citation needed] As a result, certain dyslexic deficits may be more pronounced in some orthographies than in others. For example, in alphabetic languages, phonological awareness is highly predictive of reading ability. But in Chinese (a logographic system), orthographic awareness and motor programming are highly predictive of reading ability.[8]
Type Each symbol represents Example Predictive skill Logographic word or morpheme Chinese characters Orthographic awareness, motor programming, naming speed Syllabic syllable Japanese kana Alphabetic phoneme (consonant or vowel) Latin alphabet Phonological awareness, naming speed Abugida phoneme (consonant+vowel) Indian Devanāgarī Unknown Abjad phoneme (consonant) Arabic alphabet Unknown Featural phonetic feature Korean hangul Unknown Dyslexia in alphabetic orthographies
See also: Alphabet and Managing dyslexia: alphabetic orthographyMost of the current research on dyslexia focuses on alphabetic orthography.[2]
Alphabetic writing systems vary significantly in the depth of their orthography. English and French are considered deep orthographies in comparison to Spanish and Italian which are shallow orthographies.
No alphabetic orthography is perfectly phonological; the writing systems of all alphabetic languages vary from this ideal to a greater or lesser extent.[citation needed]
Dyslexia in logographic orthographies
See also: LogogramMost research for logographic orthographies has been done for Chinese.[citation needed] Very little information is available for other logographic orthographies and their relationship to dyslexia.[citation needed]
Logographic writing systems (such as Chinese characters and Cuneiform) are significantly different from alphabetic ones. The primary difference is that their basic graphemes are logograms, a representation based on meaning (morphemes), rather than sounds (phonemes). In some logographic writing systems each character represents a single syllable; in others, each character represents a whole word.
Chinese orthographies
In alphabetic languages, phonological awareness plays a central role in reading acquisition; in Chinese, phonological awareness is much less important. Rather, reading in Chinese is strongly related to a child's writing skills, which is dependent on orthographic awareness and motor memory. In alphabetic languages with deep orthographies, the difficulty is that the child must cope with having more than one spelling to represent a sound. In spoken Chinese, a single syllable is used in many different words, and a Chinese child must cope with having many written characters that represent the same syllable.[citation needed]
Further complicating the Chinese writing system is that the Chinese character is made up of strokes and sub-character components, substantially increasing visual complexity. Thus, orthographic processing is an important aspect of reading. Deficient orthography-to-meaning mapping can lead to reading disability. A key strategy in teaching children to read is to have children repeatedly write samples of single characters, thus building the child's awareness of a character's internal structure (orthographic awareness).[8]
Rapid naming is one of the best single predictors of dyslexia in all languages tested, including both alphabetic and character-based writing systems.[8][9] There is some evidence that the means of deciphering characters differs between logographic and alphabetic writing systems differ in the brain: logographic systems echo map-reading skills.
Dyslexia in syllabic orthographies
See also: SyllabaryA syllabary is a set of written symbols that represent (or approximate) syllables, which make up words. A symbol in a syllabary typically represents an optional consonant sound followed by a vowel sound.
See also
- Linguistics
- List of writing systems
- List of languages by writing system
- Writing system
- Written language
- Medium of instruction
- English orthography
References
- ^ a b c Goswami, Usha (2005-09-06). "Chapter 28: Orthography, Phonology, and Reading Development: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective". in Malatesha, Joshi. Handbook of orthography and literacy. Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc Inc. pp. 463–464. ISBN 0805846522.
- ^ a b c Karanth, Prathibha; Jing, J (March 2002). "The search for deep dyslexia in syllabic writing systems". Journal of Neurolinguistics 15 (2): 143–155. doi:10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00022-1. PMID 19024108. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6VDV-444807H-5&_user=10&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_acct=C000050221&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=10&md5=015e4dd9555c8b8eb56797ad842337ec.
- ^ Xu GF, Jing J (September 2008). "Major achievements in relation to dyslexia in Chinese characters". Chin. Med. J. 121 (17): 1736–40. doi:10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00022-1. PMID 19024108.
- ^ Wolf, Maryanne (2007). Proust and the squid. Harper Collins. pp. 190-191. ISBN 978-0-06-018639-5.
- ^ a b Snowling, Margaret J. (2004). "Chapter 4: The science of dyslexia: a review of contemporary approaches. in Turner, Martin and John Rack. The study of dyslexia. Kluwer Academic/Plenum publishers. p. 80. ISBN 0-306-48535-4.
- ^ Landerl, K; Wimmer, H; Frith, U (1997). "The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison". Cognition 63 (3): 315–334. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00005-X. PMID 9265873.
- ^ Frith, Uta; Wimmer, Heinz; Landerl, Karin (1998). "Differences in Phonological Recoding in German- and English-Speaking Children". Scientific Studies of Reading 2 (1): 31–54. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0201_2.
- ^ a b c Tan LH, Spinks JA, Eden GF, Perfetti CA, Siok WT (June 2005). "Reading depends on writing, in Chinese". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (24): 8781–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503523102. PMC 1150863. PMID 15939871. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1150863.
- ^ Wolf, Maryanne (2007). Proust and the squid. Harper Collins. p. 178. ISBN 978-0-06-018639-5.
External links
Developmental disorders: Dyslexia and related specific developmental disorders (F80–F83, 315) General conditions Expressive language disorder · Aphasia/Dysphasia (Expressive aphasia, Receptive aphasia) · Landau–Kleffner syndrome · Lisp · Mixed receptive-expressive language disorderScholastic skills/
learning disorderDyslexia (Alexia (acquired dyslexia), Developmental dyslexia) · Dysgraphia (Disorder of written expression) · Dyscalculia (Gerstmann syndrome)Motor functionOtherRelated topics Dyslexia research · Management of dyslexia/Dyslexia interventions · Reading acquisition · Writing system · Spelling · Literacy · Irlen filters · Recording for the Blind & Dyslexic · NeuropsychologyLists Writing systems Overview Lists Types Phonologies of the world's languages Abkhaz · Arabic · Avestan · Belarusian · Bengali · Catalan · Chinese (Historical · Old · Cantonese · Standard) · Czech · Danish · Dhivehi · Dutch · English (Old · Middle · Australian · North American) · Esperanto · Finnish · French (Quebec) · German (Bernese) · Greek (Ancient · Koine) · Gujarati · Hawaiian · Hebrew (Biblical · Modern) · Hindi-Urdu · Hungarian · Icelandic · Inuit · Irish · Italian · Japanese · Kiowa · Konkani · Korean · Latin · Latvian · Macedonian · Mapudungun · Marathi · Marshallese · Navajo · Nepali · Norwegian · Occitan · Ojibwe · Ottawa · Persian · Polish · Portuguese · Proto-Indo-European · Romanian · Russian · Scottish Gaelic · Serbo-Croatian · Somali · Sotho · Spanish · Swedish · Tagalog · Tamil · Taos · Turkish · Ubykh · Ukrainian · Uyghur · Vietnamese · Welsh · Yiddish · Zuni
Categories:- Developmental dyslexia
- Dyslexia research
- Neurology
- Orthography
- Applied linguistics
- Language orthographies
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.