Bonobo

Bonobo
Bonobo[1]
Bonobos at the Cincinnati Zoo
Conservation status
Scientific classification
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Order: Primates
Family: Hominidae
Genus: Pan
Species: P. paniscus
Binomial name
Pan paniscus
Schwarz, 1929
Bonobo distribution

The bonobo (English pronunciation: /bəˈnoʊboʊ/[3][4] /ˈbɒnəboʊ/[5]), Pan paniscus, previously called the pygmy chimpanzee and less often, the dwarf or gracile chimpanzee,[6] is a great ape and one of the two species making up the genus Pan. The other species in genus Pan is Pan troglodytes, or the common chimpanzee. Although the name "chimpanzee" is sometimes used to refer to both species together, it is usually understood as referring to the common chimpanzee, while Pan paniscus is usually referred to as the bonobo.

The lifespan of a bonobo in captivity is about 40 years.[7] The lifespan in the wild is unknown.

Bonobos are far less aggressive than chimpanzees and other apes.[8]

The bonobo is endangered and is found in the wild only in the Democratic Republic of the Congo. Along with the common chimpanzee, the bonobo is the closest extant relative to humans. Because the two species are not proficient swimmers, it is possible that the formation of the Congo River 1.5–2 million years ago led to the speciation of the bonobo. They live south of the river, and thereby were separated from the ancestors of the common chimpanzee, which live north of the river.[9]

German anatomist Ernst Schwarz is credited with having discovered the bonobo in 1928, based on his analysis of a skull in the Tervuren museum in Belgium that previously had been thought to have belonged to a juvenile chimpanzee. Schwarz published his findings in 1929.[10][11] In 1933, American anatomist Harold Coolidge offered a more detailed description of the bonobo, and elevated it to species status.[11][12] The American psychologist and primatologist Robert Yerkes was also one of the first scientists to notice major differences between bonobos and chimpanzees.[13] These were first discussed in detail in a study by Eduard Paul Tratz and Heinz Heck published in the early 1950s.[14]

The species is distinguished by relatively long legs, pink lips, dark face and tail-tuft through adulthood, and parted long hair on its head.

Bonobos are perceived to be matriarchal: females tend to collectively dominate males by forming alliances; females use their sexuality to control males; a male's rank in the social hierarchy is determined by his mother's rank.[15][16] However, there are also claims of a special role for the alpha male in group movement.[citation needed] The limited research on Bonobos in the wild was also taken to indicate that these matriarchal behaviors may be exaggerated by captivity, as well as by food provisioning by researchers in the field.[15] This view has recently been challenged, however, by Duke University's Vanessa Woods;[17] Woods noted in a radio interview[18] that she had observed bonobos in a spacious forested sanctuary in the DRC exhibiting the same sort of hypersexuality under these more naturalistic conditions; and, while she acknowledges a hierarchy among males, including an "alpha male", these males are less dominant than the dominant female.

Contents

Name

Common name

The name bonobo first appeared in 1954, when Edward Tratz and Heinz Heck proposed it as a new and separate generic term for pygmy chimpanzees. The term has been reported variously[citation needed] as being a word for "chimpanzee" or "ancestor" in a Bantu language. Another suggestion for the derivation of the name is that the name is a misspelling of the name of the town of Bolobo on the Congo River, which has been associated with the collection of chimps in the 1920s.[19]

Taxonomy

The scientific name for the bonobo is Pan paniscus. While no official publication on the bonobo genome is publicly available, an initial analysis by the National Human Genome Research Institute confirmed that the bonobo genome diverges about 0.4 % from the chimpanzee genome. In addition, Svante Pääbo's group at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology is currently sequencing the genome of a female bonobo from the Leipzig zoo.[20] Initial genetic studies characterised the DNA of chimpanzees (common chimpanzee and bonobo, collectively) as being as much as 98% (99.4 in one study) identical to that of Homo sapiens.[21] Later studies showed that chimpanzees are more closely related to humans than to gorillas.[22] The most recent genetic analyses (published in 2006) of chimpanzee and human genetic similarity come from whole genome comparisons and have shown that the differences between the two species are more complex, both in extent and character, than the historical 98% figure suggests.[23]

In the seminal Nature paper reporting on initial genome comparisons, researchers identified thirty-five million single-nucleotide changes, five million insertion or deletion events, and a number of chromosomal rearrangements which constituted the genetic differences between chimpanzees and humans, covering 98% of the same genes.[24] While many of these analyses have been performed on the common chimpanzee rather than the bonobo, the differences between the two chimpanzee species are unlikely to be substantial enough to affect the Pan-Homo comparative data significantly.

There still is controversy, however. Scientists such as Jared Diamond in The Third Chimpanzee, and Morris Goodman[25] of Wayne State University in Detroit argue that the bonobo and common chimpanzee are so closely related to humans that their genus name also should be classified with the human genus Homo: Homo paniscus, Homo sylvestris, or Homo arboreus. An alternative philosophy suggests that the term Homo sapiens is the misnomer rather, and that humans should be reclassified as Pan sapiens, though this would violate the Rule of Priority as Homo was named before Pan (1758 for the former, 1816 for the latter). In either case, a name change of the genus would have implications on the taxonomy of other species closely related to humans, including Australopithecus. The current line between Homo and non-Homo species is drawn about 2 million years ago, and chimpanzee and human ancestry converges only about 7 million years ago, nearly three times earlier.

Recent DNA evidence suggests the bonobo and common chimpanzee species effectively separated from each other less than one million years ago.[26][27] The chimpanzee line split from the last common ancestor shared with humans approximately six to seven million years ago. Because no species other than Homo sapiens has survived from the human line of that branching, both Pan species are the closest living relatives of humans and cladistically are equally close to humans.

Fossils

A lot of human fossils have been found, but chimpanzee fossils were not described until 2005. Existing chimpanzee populations in West and Central Africa do not overlap with the major human fossil sites in East Africa. However, chimpanzee fossils have now been reported from Kenya. This would indicate that both humans and members of the Pan clade were present in the East African Rift Valley during the Middle Pleistocene.[28] According to A. Zihlman Bonobo body proportions closely resemble those of Australopithecus.[29] Richard Dawkins, in his book The Ancestor's Tale, proposes that chimpanzees and bonobos are descended from Australopithecus Gracile type species (see Homininae); in other words, the ancestors of chimpanzees and bonobos would be some of the Australopithecus afarensis.

Physical characteristics

Female Bonobo

The bonobo is sometimes considered to be more gracile than the common chimpanzee, and females are somewhat smaller than males. Its head is smaller than that of the common chimpanzee with less prominent brow ridges above the eyes. It has a black face with pink lips, small ears, wide nostrils, and long hair on its head that forms a part. Females have slightly more prominent breasts, in contrast to the flat breasts of other female apes, although not so prominent as those of humans. The bonobo also has a slim upper body, narrow shoulders, thin neck, and long legs when compared to the common chimpanzee.

Bonobos are both terrestrial and arboreal. Most ground locomotion is characterized by quadrupedal knuckle walking, while bipedal walking accounts for less than 1% of terrestrial locomotion.[30] These physical characteristics and its posture give the bonobo an appearance more closely resembling that of humans than that of the common chimpanzee (see: bipedal Bonobos). The bonobo also has highly individuated facial features, as humans do, so that one individual may look significantly different from another, a characteristic adapted for visual facial recognition in social interaction.

Neoteny

Multivariate analysis has shown that bonobos are more neotenized than the common chimpanzee, taking into account such features as the proportionately long torso length of the bonobo.[31] This conclusion has been challenged by other researchers.[32]

Diet

This primate is mainly frugivorous, but supplements its diet with leaves and meat from small vertebrates such as flying squirrels and duikers,[33] and invertebrates.[34] In some instances, bonobos have been shown to consume lower-order primates.[35][36] Some claim that bonobos have also been known to practice cannibalism in captivity, a claim disputed by others.[15][16] However there is at least one confirmed report of cannibalism in the wild as reported by researchers Gottfried Hohmann and Andrew Fowler.[37]

Psychological characteristics

Primatologist Frans de Waal states that bonobos are capable of altruism, compassion, empathy, kindness, patience, and sensitivity.[citation needed] How peaceful bonobos are has been disputed by some,[who?] however.

Peacefulness

Observations in the wild indicate that the males among the related common chimpanzee communities are extraordinarily hostile to males from outside the community. Parties of males 'patrol' for the unfortunate neighbouring males who might be traveling alone, and attack those single males, often killing them.[citation needed] This does not appear to be the behavior of bonobo males or females in their own communities, where they seem to prefer sexual contact over violent confrontation with outsiders. In fact, the Japanese scientists who have spent the most time working with wild bonobos describe the species as extraordinarily peaceful, and De Waal has documented how bonobos may often resolve conflicts with sexual contact (hence the "make love – not war" characterization for the species). Between groups social mingling may occur, in which members of different communities have sex and groom each other, behaviour which is unheard of among common chimpanzees. Conflict is still possible between rival groups of bonobos, but no official scientific reports of it exist. The ranges of bonobos and chimpanzees are separated by the Congo River with bonobos living south of the river and chimpanzees living north of the river.[38] It has been hypothesized that bonobos are able to live a more peaceful lifestyle in part because of an abundance of nutritious vegetation in their natural habitat, allowing them to travel and forage in large parties.[39]

The popular image of the bonobo as a peaceful ape does not always apply to captive populations. Accounts exist of bonobos confined in zoos mutilating one another and engaging in bullying. These incidents may be due to the practice in zoos of separating mothers and sons, which is contrary to their social organization in the wild. Bonobo society is dominated by females, and severing the lifelong alliance between mothers and their male offspring may make them vulnerable to female aggression. De Waal has warned of the danger of romanticizing bonobos: "All animals are competitive by nature and cooperative only under specific circumstances" as well as writing that "when first writing about their behavior, I spoke of 'sex for peace' precisely because bonobos had plenty of conflicts. There would obviously be no need for peacemaking if they lived in perfect harmony". There is no eyewitness account of lethal aggression among bonobos, neither in captivity nor in the wild.

Hohmann and Surbeck published in 2008 that bonobos sometimes do hunt monkey species. Five incidents were observed in a group of bonobos in Salonga National Park, which seemed to reflect deliberate cooperative hunting. On three occasions, the hunt was successful and infant monkeys were captured. This behavior falls under feeding, however, not aggression.

Social behavior

Bonobo fishing for termites

Most studies indicate that females have a higher social status in bonobo society. Aggressive encounters between males and females are rare, and males are tolerant of infants and juveniles. A male's status is derived from the status of his mother. The mother-son bond often stays strong and continues throughout life. While social hierarchies do exist, rank plays a less prominent role than in other primate societies.

Bonobo party size tends to vary because the groups exhibit a fission-fusion pattern. A community of approximately 100 will split into small groups during the day while looking for food, and then comes back together to sleep. They sleep in trees in nests that they construct.

Sexual social behavior

Sexual intercourse plays a major role in bonobo society observed in captivity, being used as what some scientists perceive as a greeting, a means of forming social bonds, a means of conflict resolution, and post-conflict reconciliation. Bonobos are the only non-human animal to have been observed engaging in all of the following sexual activities: face-to-face genital sex (although a pair of Western Gorillas has been photographed performing face-to-face genital sex[40]), tongue kissing, and oral sex.[41] In scientific literature, the female-female behavior of touching genitals together is often referred to as GG rubbing or genital-genital rubbing, or, as referred to commonly by British primate researchers, "scissoring." The sexual activity happens within the immediate community and sometimes outside of it. Bonobos do not form permanent monogamous sexual relationships with individual partners. They also do not seem to discriminate in their sexual behavior by sex or age, with the possible exception of abstaining from sexual intercourse between mothers and their adult sons. When bonobos come upon a new food source or feeding ground, the increased excitement will usually lead to communal sexual activity, presumably decreasing tension and encouraging peaceful feeding.[42]

Group of Bonobos

Bonobo males occasionally engage in various forms of male-male genital behavior.[43][44] In one form, two males hang from a tree limb face-to-face while "penis fencing".[45][46] This also may occur when two males rub their penises together while in face-to-face position. Another form of genital interaction, called "rump rubbing", occurs to express reconciliation between two males after a conflict, when they stand back-to-back and rub their scrotal sacs together. Takayoshi Kano observed similar practices among bonobos in the natural habitat.

Bonobo females also engage in female-female genital behavior, possibly to bond socially with each other, thus forming a female nucleus of bonobo society. The bonding among females enables them to dominate bonobo society. Although male bonobos are individually stronger, they cannot stand alone against a united group of females.[46] Adolescent females often leave their native community to join another community. Sexual bonding with other females establishes these new females as members of the group. This migration mixes the bonobo gene pools, providing genetic diversity.

Bonobo reproductive rates are not any higher than those of the common chimpanzee.[42] Female bonobos carry and nurse their young for five years and can give birth every five to six years. Compared to common chimpanzees, bonobo females resume the genital swelling cycle much sooner after giving birth, enabling them to rejoin the sexual activities of their society. Also, bonobo females who are sterile or too young to reproduce still engage in sexual activity.

Similarity to humans

Bonobos are capable of passing the mirror-recognition test for self-awareness.[47] They communicate primarily through vocal means, although the meanings of their vocalizations are not currently known. However, most humans do understand their facial expressions[21] and some of their natural hand gestures, such as their invitation to play. Two Bonobos at the Great Ape Trust, Kanzi and Panbanisha, have been taught how to communicate using a keyboard labeled with lexigrams (geometric symbols) and they can respond to spoken sentences. Kanzi's vocabulary consists of more than 500 English words[48] and he has comprehension of around 3,000 spoken English words.[49] Kanzi has also been known for learning from observation of people trying to teach his mother. His mother was not learning some things and Kanzi started doing the tasks that his mother was taught just by watching. Some, such as philosopher and bioethicist Peter Singer, argue that these results qualify them for the "rights to survival and life", rights that humans theoretically accord to all persons.

There are instances in which non-human primates have been reported to have expressed joy. One study analyzed and recorded sounds made by human babies and Bonobos when they were tickled.[50] It found although the Bonobo's laugh was a higher frequency, the laugh followed a similar spectrographic pattern to human babies.[50]

Habitat

Bonobos are found only south of the Congo River and north of the Kasai River (a tributary of the Congo),[51] in the humid forests of the Democratic Republic of Congo of central Africa.

Conservation efforts

Vanessa Woods with bonobo at Lola ya Bonobo sanctuary in Congo

The IUCN Red List classifies bonobos as an endangered species with conservative population estimates ranging from 29,500 to 50,000 individuals.[52] Major threats to bonobo populations include habitat loss and hunting for bushmeat, the latter activity having increased dramatically during the first and second Congo wars in the Democratic Republic of Congo due to the presence of heavily armed militias even in remote "protected" areas such as Salonga National Park. This is part of a more general trend of ape extinction.

As the bonobo's habitat is shared with people, the ultimate success of conservation efforts will rely on local and community involvement. The issue of parks vs. people[53] is salient in the Cuvette Centrale, the bonobo's range. There is strong local and broad-based Congolese resistance to establishing national parks, as indigenous communities often have been driven from their forest homes by the establishment of parks. In Salonga National Park, the only national park in the bonobo habitat, there is no local involvement, and recent surveys[when?] indicate that the bonobo, the African Forest Elephant, and other species have been severely devastated by poachers and the thriving bushmeat trade. In contrast to this, there are areas where the bonobo and biodiversity still thrive without any established parks, due to the indigenous beliefs and taboos against killing bonobos.

The port town of Basankusu is situated on the Lulonga River, at the confluence of the Lopori and Maringa Rivers, in the north of the country, making it well placed to receive and transport local goods to the cities of Mbandaka and Kinshasa. With Basankusu being the last port of substance before the wilderness of the Lopori Basin and the Lomako River – the bonobo heartland, conservation efforts for the bonobo,[54] use the town as a base.[55][56]

In 1995, concern over declining numbers of Bonobos in the wild led the Zoological Society of Milwaukee in Milwaukee, Wisconsin, with contributions from Bonobo scientists around the world, to publish the Action Plan for Pan paniscus: A Report on Free Ranging Populations and Proposals for their Preservation. The Action Plan compiles population data on Bonobos from twenty years of research conducted at various sites throughout the Bonobo's range. The plan identifies priority actions for Bonobo conservation and serves as a reference for developing conservation programs for researchers, government officials, and donor agencies.

Acting on Action Plan recommendations, the ZSM developed the Bonobo and Congo Biodiversity Initiative (BCBI). This program includes habitat and rain-forest preservation, training for Congolese nationals and conservation institutions, wildlife population assessment and monitoring, and education. The Zoological Society has conducted regional surveys within the range of the bonobo in conjunction with training Congolese researchers in survey methodology and biodiversity monitoring. The Zoological Society’s initial goal was to survey Salonga National Park to determine the conservation status of the bonobo within the park and to provide financial and technical assistance to strengthen park protection. As the project has developed, the Zoological Society has become more involved in helping the Congolese living in bonobo habitat. The Zoological Society has built schools, hired teachers, provided some medicines, and, as of 2007, started an agriculture project to help the Congolese learn to grow crops and depend less on hunting wild animals.

During the wars in the 1990s, researchers and international non-governmental organizations (NGOs) were driven out of the Bonobo habitat. In 2002, the Bonobo Conservation Initiative initiated the Bonobo Peace Forest Project in cooperation with national institutions, local NGOs, and local communities. The Peace Forest Project works with local communities to establish a linked constellation of community-based reserves, managed by local and indigenous people. Although there has been only limited support from international organizations, this model, implemented mainly through DRC organizations and local communities, has helped bring about agreements to protect over 5,000 square miles (13,000 km2) of the bonobo habitat. According to Dr. Amy Parish, the Bonobo Peace Forest "…is going to be a model for conservation in the 21st century."[57]

This initiative has been gaining momentum and greater international recognition and it recently[when?] has gained greater support through Conservation International, the Global Conservation Fund, United States Fish and Wildlife Service's Great Ape Conservation Fund, and the United Nations' Great Apes Survival Project.

With grants from the United Nations, USAID, the U.S. Embassy, the World Wildlife Fund, and many other groups and individuals, the Zoological Society also has been working to:

  • Survey the Bonobo population and its habitat in order to find ways to help protect these apes.
  • Develop anti-poaching measures to help save apes, forest elephants, and other endangered animals in Congo's Salonga National Park, a U.N. World Heritage Site.
  • Provide training, literacy education, agricultural techniques, schools, equipment, and jobs for Congolese living near Bonobo habitats so that they will have a vested interest in protecting the great apes. As of 2007, the ZSM started an agriculture project to help the Congolese learn to grow crops and depend less on hunting wild animals.
  • Model small-scale conservation methods that can be used throughout Congo.

Starting in 2003, the U.S. government allocated $54 million to the Congo Basin Forest Partnership. This significant investment has triggered the involvement of international NGOs to establish bases in the region and work to develop bonobo conservation programs. This initiative should improve the likelihood of Bonobo survival, but its success still may depend upon building greater involvement and capability in local and indigenous communities.[58]

The Congo is setting aside more than 11,000 square miles (28,000 km2) of rain forest to help protect the endangered Bonobo, in this central African country. U.S. agencies, conservation groups, and the Congolese government have come together to set aside 11,803 square miles (30,570 km2) of tropical rain forest, the U.S.-based Bonobo Conservation Initiative. The area amounts to just over 1% of the vast Congo – but that means a park larger than the state of Massachusetts.

The bonobo population is believed to have declined sharply in the last thirty years, though surveys have been hard to carry out in war-ravaged central Congo. Estimates range from 60,000 to fewer than 50,000 living, according to the World Wildlife Fund.

The Sankuru reserve also contains Okapi, closely related to the Giraffe, that also is native to Congo, elephants, and at least ten other primate species.

In addition, concerned parties have addressed the crisis on several science and ecological websites. Organizations such as the World Wide Fund for Nature, the African Wildlife Foundation, and others are trying to focus attention on the extreme risk to the species. Some have suggested that a reserve be established in a more stable part of Africa, or on an island in a place such as Indonesia. Awareness is ever increasing and even non-scientific or ecological sites have created various groups to collect donations to help with the conservation of this species.

See also

Okapi2.jpg Mammals portal

References

  1. ^ Groves, C. (2005). Wilson, D. E., & Reeder, D. M, eds. ed. Mammal Species of the World (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 183. OCLC 62265494. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. http://www.bucknell.edu/msw3. 
  2. ^ Fruth, B., Benishay, J.M., Bila-Isia, I., Coxe, S., Dupain, J., Furuichi, T., Hart, J., Hart, T., Hashimoto, C., Hohmann, G., Hurley, M., Ilambu, O., Mulavwa, M., Ndunda, M., Omasombo, V., Reinartz, G., Scherlis, J., Steel, L. & Thompson, J. (2008). Pan paniscus. In: IUCN 2008. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Downloaded on 4 January 2009. Listed as Endangered (EN A4cd v3.1)
  3. ^ Merriam Webster (listen),
  4. ^ "dictionary.com". Dictionary.reference.com. http://dictionary.reference.com/browse/bonobo. Retrieved 2009-07-03. 
  5. ^ Merriam Webster (listen),
  6. ^ Frans de Waal, Frans Lanting (1997) Bonobo: The Forgotten Ape, University of California Press
  7. ^ Rowe N. Pictural Guide to the Living Primates. Pogonias Press, 1996. East Hampton. 263pp (ISBN 0964882515)
  8. ^ Aggression topics form the University of New Hampshire
  9. ^ Caswell JL, Mallick S, Richter DJ, et al (2008). McVean, Gil. ed. "Analysis of Chimpanzee History Based on Genome Sequence Alignments". PLoS Genet. 4 (4): e1000057. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000057. PMC 2278377. PMID 18421364. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2278377. 
  10. ^ Schwarz, Ernst: "Das Vorkommen des Schimpansen auf den linken Kongo-Ufer", Rev. Zool. Bot. Afr. 16, pp. 425–426, April 1, 1929.
  11. ^ a b Coolidge, Harold Jefferson Jr.: "Pan paniscus. Pigmy chimpanzee from south of the Congo river", American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 18(1), pp. 1–59; July/September 1933. Coolidge's paper contains a translation of Schwarz's earlier report. URL last accessed 2011-01-21.
  12. ^ See Herzfeld, Chris (2007), « L’invention du bonobo », Bulletin d’histoire et d’épistémologie des sciences de la vie, volume 14, numéro 2, 2007.
  13. ^ CultureLab: Bonobos have a secret. Newscientist.com (2010-06-21). Retrieved on 2011-02-15.
  14. ^ Frans B. M. Waal, Tree of Origin: What Primate Behavior Can Tell Us About Human Social Evolution, Harvard University Press, 2002, p. 51
  15. ^ a b c Parker, Ian (2009-01-07). "Our Far-Flung Correspondents: Swingers: Reporting & Essays". The New Yorker. http://www.newyorker.com/reporting/2007/07/30/070730fa_fact_parker. Retrieved 2009-07-03. 
  16. ^ a b de Waal, Frans (2009-10-18). "Was "Ardi" a Liberal?". The Huffington Post. http://www.huffingtonpost.com/frans-de-waal/was-ardi-perhaps-liberal_b_325201.html. Retrieved 2009-10-18. 
  17. ^ Research, Vanessa Woods
  18. ^ #61 Bonobo Handshake. Skepticallyspeaking.com. Retrieved on 2011-02-15.
  19. ^ Sue Savage-Rumbaugh & Roger Lewin (1994). Kanzi: the ape at the brink of the human mind. John Wiley & Sons. p. 97. ISBN 0385403321. 
  20. ^ By Julia Karow. Neandertal, Bonobo Genomes May Shed Light On Human Evolution; MPI, 454 Preparing Drafts. May 13, 2008
  21. ^ a b "Colombus Zoo: Bonobo". Archived from the original on 2006-05-02. http://web.archive.org/web/20060502101116/http://www.colszoo.org/animalareas/aforest/bonobo.html. Retrieved 2006-08-01. 
  22. ^ Takahata, N.; Satta, Y. & Klein, J. (1995). "Divergence time and population size in the lineage leading to modern humans". Theor Popul Biol 48 (2): 198–221. doi:10.1006/tpbi.1995.1026. PMID 7482371. 
  23. ^ "Human-Chimp Gene Gap Widens from Tally of Duplicate Genes". http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=human-chimp-gene-gap-wide. Retrieved 2010-02-25. 
  24. ^ The Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium (1 September 2005). "Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome". Nature 437 (7055): 69–87. doi:10.1038/nature04072. PMID 16136131. 
  25. ^ Hecht, Jeff (2003-05-19). "Chimps are human, gene study implies". New Scientist. 
  26. ^ Won, Yong-Jin et al. (2004-10-13). "Divergence Population Genetics of Chimpanzees". Molecular Biology & Evolution 22 (2): 297–307. doi:10.1093/molbev/msi017. PMID 15483319. 
  27. ^ Fischer, Anne et al. (2004-02-12). "Evidence for a Complex Demographic History of Chimpanzees". Molecular Biology & Evolution 21 (5): 799–808. doi:10.1093/molbev/msh083. PMID 14963091. 
  28. ^ McBrearty, S.; N. G. Jablonski (2005-09-01). "First fossil chimpanzee". Nature 437 (7055): 105–8. doi:10.1038/nature04008. PMID 16136135. 
  29. ^ Zihlman AL, Cronin JE, Cramer DL, Sarich VM. 1978, Pygmy chimpanzee as a possible prototype for the common ancestor of humans, chimpanzees and gorillas. Nature. 275: 744-6.. PMID: 703839
  30. ^ Doran (1993). "Comparative locomotor behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos: the influence of morphology on locomotion". Am J Phys Anthropol. 91 (1):  :83–98. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330910106. PMID 8512056. 
  31. ^ Shea, B.T. (1983) Paedomorphosis and neoteny in the pygmy chimpanzee. Science 4 November 1983: 521-522
  32. ^ Godfrey L, Sutherland M. Paradox of peramophic paedomorphosis: heterochrony and human evolution. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1996;99:17–42
  33. ^ Ihobe H (1992). "Observations on the meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire". Primates 33 (2): 247–250. doi:10.1007/BF02382754. http://www.springerlink.com/content/h7145027g60n708l/. 
  34. ^ Rafert, J. and E.O. Vineberg (1997). "Bonobo Nutrition – relation of captive diet to wild diet," Bonobo Husbandry Manual, American Association of Zoos and Aquariums
  35. ^ Surbeck M, Fowler A, Deimel C, Hohmann G (2008). "Evidence for the consumption of arboreal, diurnal primates by bonobos (Pan paniscus)". American Journal of Primatology 71 (2): 171–4. doi:10.1002/ajp.20634. PMID 19058132. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/121543963/abstract. 
  36. ^ Surbeck M, Hohmann G (14 October 2008). "Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park". Current Biology 18 (19): R906–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.040. PMID 18957233. http://www.current-biology.com/content/article/abstract?uid=PIIS0960982208011172. 
  37. ^ "Bonobo 'cannibalises' own infant". BBC News. 2010-02-01. http://news.bbc.co.uk/earth/hi/earth_news/newsid_8487000/8487138.stm. 
  38. ^ The Smart and Swinging Bonobo|Science & Nature|Smithsonian Magazine. Smithsonianmag.com. Retrieved on 2011-02-15.
  39. ^ White & Wrangham (1988) "Feeding competition and patch size in the chimpanzee species Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes." Behaviour 105:148-164.
  40. ^ Tuan C. Nguyen, "Gorillas Caught in Very Human Act" in Live Science (February 13, 2008) http://www.livescience.com/2298-gorillas-caught-human-act.html
  41. ^ Manson, J.H.; Perry, S.; Parish, A.R. (1997). "Nonconceptive Sexual Behavior in Bonobos and Capuchins". International Journal of Primatology 18 (5): 767–86. doi:10.1023/A:1026395829818. 
  42. ^ a b Frans B. M. de Waal (March 1995). "Bonobo Sex and Society". Scientific American. pp. 82–8. http://songweaver.com/info/bonobos.html. Retrieved 2006-07-17. 
  43. ^ Frans de Waal, "Bonobo Sex and Society" in Scientific American (March 1995), p. 82ff
  44. ^ "Courtney Laird, "Social Organization"". Bio.davidson.edu. 2004. http://www.bio.davidson.edu/people/vecase/Behavior/Spring2004/laird/Social%20Organization.htm. Retrieved 2009-07-03. 
  45. ^ Frans B. M. de Waal (2001). "Bonobos and Fig Leaves". The ape and the sushi master : cultural reflections by a primatologist. Basic Books. ISBN 8449313252. 
  46. ^ a b de Waal, Frans B.M. (March 1995). "Bonobo Sex and Society". Scientific American 272 (3). http://primate.uchicago.edu/DeWaalSciAm.doc. 
  47. ^ Miller, Jason (2009). "Minding the Animals: Ethology and the Obsolescence of Left Humanism". American Chronicle. http://www.americanchronicle.com/articles/view/102661. Retrieved 2009-05-21. 
  48. ^ "Meet our Great Apes: Kanzi". Archived from the original on 2008-06-30. http://web.archive.org/web/20080630201101/http://www.greatapetrust.org/bonobo/meet/kanzi.php. Retrieved 2008-09-28. 
  49. ^ Raffaele, P. (2006). "Speaking Bonobo". Smithsonian. http://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/10022981.html. Retrieved 2008-09-28. 
  50. ^ a b Beale, B. (2003). "Where Did Laughter Come From?". ABC Science Online. http://dsc.discovery.com/news/afp/20031006/laughter.html. Retrieved 2008-08-10. 
  51. ^ Dawkins, Richard (2004). "Chimpanzees". The Ancestor's Tale. Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 115516265X. 
  52. ^ http://www.iucnredlist.org/apps/redlist/details/15932/0
  53. ^ Reid, John, Parks and people, not parks vs. people, San Francisco Chronicle, June 15, 2006
  54. ^ Searching for Bonobo – Dr Therese Hart
  55. ^ Lola Ya Bonobo (Bonobo Heaven)
  56. ^ Bonobo Reintroduction in the Democratic Republic of Congo
  57. ^ "The Make Love, Not War Species", Living on Earth, (July 2006), National Public Radio
  58. ^ Chapin, Mac, (November/December 2004), Vision for a Sustainable World, WORLDWATCH magazine

Further reading

Books

Articles

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать реферат

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Bonobo — (Pan paniscus) Systematik Unterordnung: Trockennasenaffen (Haplorhini) Teilordnung: Altweltaff …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Bonobo — Saltar a navegación, búsqueda Bonobo puede referirse a: Bonobo, modelo de componentes software; Bonobo, primate similar al chimpancé; Bonobo, DJ británico del sello Ninja Tune. Obtenido de Bonobo Categoría: Wikipedia:Desambiguación …   Wikipedia Español

  • bonobo — o*no bo (b[ o]*n[=o] b[=o]), n. an anthropoid ape ({Pan paniscus}), resembling but smaller than the common chimpanzee ({Pan troglodytes}); called also {pygmy chimpanzee}. It is found in the forests of Zaire. Syn: pygmy chimpanzee, pygmy chimp.… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • bonobo — ● bonobo nom masculin Chimpanzé des forêts de la rive gauche du fleuve Congo, moins corpulent que le chimpanzé commun, mais au comportement social plus affirmé …   Encyclopédie Universelle

  • Bonobo — (Pan paniscus) er en af menneskeaberne og den er også kendt som pygmæ chimpanse . Bonoboer blev opdaget i 1928 af den amerikanske anatom Harold Coolidge. Sammen med chimpanserne er bonoboerne menneskets nærmeste slægtninge. Bonoboerne viste i… …   Danske encyklopædi

  • bonobo — [bə nō′bō] n. pl. bonobos a small, long limbed chimpanzee (Pan paniscus) of the rain forests of Democratic Republic of the Congo …   English World dictionary

  • Bonobo — Pour les articles homonymes, voir Bonobo (homonymie). Bonobo …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Bonobo — У этого термина существуют и другие значения, см. Bonobo (GNOME). Bonobo …   Википедия

  • bonobo — /beuh noh boh/, n., pl. bonobos. See pygmy chimpanzee. * * * Species (Pan paniscus) of great ape. It was once considered a subspecies of the chimpanzee, which it closely resembles in size, appearance, and way of life. Its range, the lowland… …   Universalium

  • Bonobo — La palabra Bonobo puede referirse a: ● El modelo de componentes software Bonobo. ● El chimpancé Pan paniscus. * * * o chimpancé enano Especie (Pan paniscus) de los grandes simios. Fue considerado una subespecie del chimpancé, al que se parece… …   Enciclopedia Universal

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”