Priming (psychology)

Priming (psychology)

Priming is an implicit memory effect in which exposure to a stimulus influences a response to a later stimulus. It can occur following perceptual, semantic, or conceptual stimulus repetition. For example, if a person reads a list of words including the word table, and is later asked to complete a word starting with tab, the probability that he or she will answer table is greater than if not so primed. Another example is if people see an incomplete sketch that they are unable to identify and they are shown more of the sketch until they recognize the picture, later they will identify the sketch at an earlier stage than was possible for them the first time.[1]

The effects of priming can be very salient and long lasting, even more so than simple recognition memory.[2] Unconscious priming effects can affect word choice on a word-stem completion test long after the words have been consciously forgotten.[2]

Priming works best when the two stimuli are in the same modality. For example visual priming works best with visual cues and verbal priming works best with verbal cues. But priming also occurs between modalities,[3] or between semantically related words such as "doctor" and "nurse".[4]

Contents

Types

Positive and negative priming

The terms positive and negative priming refer to when priming affects the speed of processing. A positive prime speeds up processing, while a negative prime lowers the speed to slower than un-primed levels.[5] Positive priming is caused by simply experiencing the stimulus,[6] while negative priming is caused by experiencing the stimulus, and then ignoring it.[5] Positive priming effects happen even if the prime is not consciously seen.[6] The effects of positive and negative priming are visible in event-related potential (ERP) readings.[7]

Positive priming is thought to be caused by spreading activation.[6] This means that the first stimulus activates parts of a particular representation or association in memory just before carrying out an action or task. The representation is already partially activated when the second stimulus is encountered, so less additional activation is needed for one to become consciously aware of it.

Negative priming is more difficult to explain. Many models have been hypothesized, but currently the most widely accepted are the distractor inhibition and episodic retrieval models.[5] In the distractor inhibition model, the activation of ignored stimuli is inhibited by the brain.[5] The episodic retrieval model hypothesizes that ignored items are flagged 'do-not-respond' by the brain. Later, when the brain acts to retrieve this information, the tag causes a conflict. The time taken to resolve this conflict causes negative priming.[5]

Perceptual and conceptual priming

The difference between perceptual and conceptual primes is whether items with a similar form or items with a similar meaning are primed.

Perceptual priming is based on the form of the stimulus and is enhanced by the match between the early and later stimuli. Perceptual priming is sensitive to the modality and exact format of the stimulus. An example of perceptual priming is the identification of an incomplete word in a word-stem completion test. The presentation of the visual prime does not have to be perfectly consistent with later testing presentations in order to work. Studies have shown that, for example, the absolute size of the stimuli can vary and still provide significant evidence of priming [8]

Conceptual priming is based on the meaning of a stimulus and is enhanced by semantic tasks. For example, table, will show priming effects on chair, because table and chair belong to the same category.[9]

Repetition

Repetition, also called direct priming is a form of positive priming. When a stimulus is experienced, it is also primed. This means that later experiences of the stimulus will be processed more quickly by the brain.[10] This effect has been found on words in the lexical decision task.

Semantic

In semantic priming, the prime and the target are from the same semantic category and share features.[11] For example, the word dog is a semantic prime for wolf, because the two are both similar animals. Semantic priming is theorized to work because of spreading neural networks.[6] When a person thinks of one item in a category, similar items are stimulated by the brain. Even if they are not words, morphemes can prime for complete words that include them.[12] An example of this would be that the morpheme 'psych' can prime for the word 'psychology'.

Associative

In associative priming, the target is a word that has a high probability of appearing with the prime, and is "associated" with it but not necessarily related in semantic features. dog is an associative prime for cat, since the words are closely associated and frequently appear together (in phrases like "raining cats and dogs").[13] A similar effect is known as context priming. Context priming works by using a context to speed up processing for stimuli that are likely to occur in that context. A useful application of this effect is reading written text.[14] The grammar and vocabulary of the sentence provide contextual clues for words that will occur later in the sentence. These later words are processed more quickly than if they had been read alone, and the effect is greater for more difficult or uncommon words.[14]

Response Priming

In the psychology of visual perception and motor control, the term Response Priming denotes a special form of visuomotor priming effect. The distinctive feature of response priming is that prime and target are presented in quick succession (typically, less than 100 milliseconds apart) and are coupled to identical or alternative motor responses.[15][16] When a speeded motor response is performed to classify the target stimulus, a prime immediately preceding the target can thus induce response conflicts when assigned to a different response as the target. These response conflicts have observable effects on motor behavior, leading to priming effects, e.g., in response times and error rates. A special property of response priming is its independence from visual awareness of the prime: For example, response priming effects can increase under conditions where visual awareness of the prime is decreasing.[17][18]

Masked priming: Examining orthographic and phonological activation during word recognition

The masked priming paradigm has been widely used in the last two decades in order to investigate both orthographic and phonological activations during visual word recognition. The term "masked" refers to the fact that the prime word or pseudoword is masked by symbols such as ###### that can be presented in a forward manner (before the prime) or a backward manner (after the prime). These masks enable to diminish the visibility of the prime. The prime is usually presented less than 80 ms in this paradigm. In all, the short SOA (duration of prime presentation) associated with the masking make the masked priming paradigm a good tool to investigate automatic and irrepressive activations during visual word recognition.

Measuring the effects of priming

Priming effects can be found with many of the tests of implicit memory. Tests such as the word-stem completion task, and the word fragment completion task measure perceptual priming. In the word-stem completion task, participants are given a list of study words, and then asked to complete word "stems" consisting of 3 letters with the first word that comes to mind. A priming effect is observed when participants complete stems with words on the study list more often than with novel words. The word fragment completion task is similar, but instead of being given the stem of a word, participants are given a word with some letters missing. The lexical decision task can be used to demonstrate conceptual priming. In this task, participants are asked to determine if a given string is a word or a nonword. Priming is demonstrated when participants are quicker to respond to words that have been primed with semantically-related words, e.g., faster to confirm "nurse" as a word when it is preceded by "doctor" than when it is preceded by "butter". Other evidence has been found through brain imaging and studies from brain injured patients.

Effects of brain injuries

Amnesia

Amnesic patients are described as those who have suffered damage to their medial temporal lobe, resulting in the impairment of explicit recollection of everyday facts and events. Priming studies on amnesic patients have varying results, depending on both the type of priming test done, as well as the phrasing of the instructions.

Amnesic patients do as well on perceptual priming tasks as healthy patients,[19] however they show some difficulties completing conceptual priming tasks, depending on the specific test. For example, they perform normally on category instance production tasks, but show impaired priming on any task that involves answering general knowledge questions.[20][21]

Phrasing of the instructions associated with the test has a dramatic impact on an amnesic's ability to complete the task successfully. When performing a word-stem completion test, patients were able to successfully complete the task when asked to complete the stem using the first word that came to mind, but when explicitly asked to recall a word to complete the stem that was on the study list, patients performed at below-average levels.[22]

Overall, studies from amnesic patients indicate that priming is controlled by a brain system separate from the medial temporal system that supports explicit memory.

Aphasia

Perhaps the first use of semantic priming in neurological patients was with stroke patients with aphasia. In one study, patients with Wernicke's aphasia who were unable to make semantic judgments showed evidence of semantic priming, while patient with Broca's aphasia who were able to make semantic judgments showed less consistent priming than Wernicke's aphasics or normal controls (Milberg and Blumstein 1981). This dissociation was extended to other linguistic categories such phonology and syntactic processing by Blumstein, Milberg and their colleagues.[citation needed]

Dementia

Patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), the most common form of dementia, have been studied extensively as far as priming goes. Results are conflicting in some cases, but overall, AD patients show decreased priming effects on word-stem completion and free association tasks, while retaining normal performance on lexical decision tasks.[23] These results suggest that AD patients are impaired in any sort of priming task that requires semantic processing of the stimuli, while priming tasks that require visuoperceptual interpretation of stimuli are unaffected by Alzheimers.

Focal cortical lesions

Patient J.P., who suffered a stroke in the left medial/temporal gyrus, resulting in word meaning deafness - the inability to comprehend spoken words, but maintaining the ability to read and write, and with no effects to hearing ability. J.P. showed normal perceptual priming, but his conceptual priming ability for spoken words was, expectedly, impaired.[24] Another patient, N.G., who suffered from prosopanomia (the inability to retrieve proper names) following damage the his left temporal lobe, was unable to spontaneously provide names of persons or cities, but was able to successfully complete a word-fragment completion exercise following priming with these names. This demonstrated intact perceptual priming abilities.[25]

Neurology

Perceptual priming

Example alt text
The extrastriate cortex (shown in orange and red) is believed to be involved in perceptual priming

Priming while improving performance decreases neural processing in the cerebral cortex of sensory stimuli with stimulus repetition. This has been found in single-cell recordings[26] and in electroencephalography (EEG) upon gamma waves,[27] with PET [28] and functional MRI.[29] This reduction is due to representational sharpening in the early sensory areas which reduces the number of neurons representing the stimulus. This leads to a more selective activation of neurons representing objects in higher cognitive areas.[30]

Conceptual priming

Conceptual priming has been linked to reduced blood flow in the left prefrontal cortex.[31] The left prefrontal cortex is believed to be involved in the semantic processing of words, among other tasks.[32]

The view that perceptual priming is controlled by the extrastriate cortex while conceptual priming is controlled by the left prefrontal cortex is undoubtedly an oversimplified view of the process, and current work is focused on elucidating the brain regions involved in priming in more detail.[33]

In daily life

Priming is thought to play a large part in the systems of stereotyping.[34] This is because attention to a response increases the frequency of that response, even if the attended response is undesired.[34][35] The attention given to these response or behaviours primes them for later activation.[34]

This can occur even if the subject is not conscious of the priming stimulus.[34] An example of this was done by Bargh et al. in 1996. Subjects were implicitly primed with words related to the stereotype of elderly people (example: Florida, forgetful, wrinkle). While the words did not explicitly mention speed or slowness, those who were primed with these words walked more slowly upon exiting the testing booth than those who were primed with neutral stimuli.[34] Similar effects were found with rude and polite stimuli: those primed with rude words were more likely to interrupt an investigator than those primed with neutral words, and those primed with polite words were the least likely to interrupt.[34] A Harvard study showed that something as simple as holding a hot or cold beverage before an interview could result in pleasant or negative opinion of the interviewer.[36]

References

  1. ^ Kolb & Whishaw: Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (2003), page 453-454, 457.
  2. ^ a b Tulving, Endel; Daniel L. Schacter; Heather A. Stark (1982). "Priming Effects in Word Fragment Completion are independent of Recognition Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 8 (4). 
  3. ^ Several researchers, for example, have used cross-modal priming to investigate syntactic deficits in individuals with damage to Broca's area of the brain. See the following:
    • Zurif E.B., Swinney D., Prather P., Solomon J., Bushell C. (1993). "An on-line analysis of syntactic processing in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia". Brain and Language 45 (3): 448–464. doi:10.1006/brln.1993.1054. PMID 8269334. 
    • Swinney, D., E. Zurif, P. Prather, and T. Love (1993). "The neurological distribution of processing operations underlying language comprehension." Manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego.
    • For an overview, see also Zurif, E.B. (1995), "Brain Regions of Relevance to Syntactic Processing." in Knowledge of Meaning: An Introduction to Semantic Theory, eds. Richard Larson and Gabriel Segal. MIT Press.
  4. ^ Friederici, Angela D.; Karsten Steinhauer and Stefan Frisch (1999). "Lexical integration: Sequential effects of syntactic and semantic information". Memory & Cognition 27 (3): 438–453. doi:10.3758/BF03211539. "Semantic priming refers to the finding that word recognition is typically faster when the target word (e.g., doctor) is preceded by a semantically related prime word (e.g., nurse)." 
  5. ^ a b c d e Mayr, Susanne; Axel Buchner (2007). "Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon: A Review of 20 Years of Negative Priming Research". Journal of Psychology 215 (1): 35. doi:10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.35. 
  6. ^ a b c d Reisberg, Daniel: Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind (2007), page 255, 517.
  7. ^ Bentin, Shlomo; Gregory McCarthy; Charles C. Wood (1985). "Event Related Potentials, Lexical Decision and Semantic Priming". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 60. 
  8. ^ Beiderman, Irving; Eric E. Cooper (1992). "Size Invariance in Visual Object Priming". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18 (1). 
  9. ^ Vaidya, Chandan L; Laura A Monti; John DE Gabrieli; Jared R Tinklenburg; Jerome A Yesevage (1999). "Dissociation between two forms of conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease". Neuropsychology 13 (4): 516–24. PMID 10527059. http://svalz.stanford.edu/docs/paper14.pdf. 
  10. ^ Forster, Kenneth I; Chris Davis (1984). "Repetition Priming and Frequency Attenuation". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 10 (4). 
  11. ^ Ludovic Ferrand and Boris New: Semantic and associative priming in the mental lexicon, found on: boris.new.googlepages.com/Semantic-final-2003.pdf
  12. ^ Marslen-Wilson, William; Lorraine Komisarjevsky Tyler; Rachelle Waksler (1994). "Morphology and Meaning in the English Mental Lexicon". Psychological Review 215 (1). 
  13. ^ Matsukawa, Junko; Joan Gay Snodgrass; Glen M. Doniger (2005). "Conceptual versus perceptual priming in incomplete picture identification". Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34 (6). 
  14. ^ a b Stanovich, Keith E.; Richard F. West (1983). "On Priming by a Sentence Context". Journal of Experimental Psychology 112 (1). 
  15. ^ Klotz, W., & Wolff, P. : The effect of a masked stimulus on the response to the masking stimulus. In: Psychological Research, Nr. 58, 1995, p. 92-101.
  16. ^ Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. : Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking. In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Nr. 25, 1999, p. 976-992.
  17. ^ Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J.: Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, Nr. 100, 2003, p. 6275-6280.
  18. ^ Schmidt, T., & Vorberg, D.: Criteria for unconscious cognition: Three types of dissociation. In: Perception & Psychophysics, Nr. 68, 2006, p. 489-504.
  19. ^ Cermak, L.S.; Talbot, N.; Chandler, K.; Wolbarst, L.R. (1985). "The perceptual priming phenomenon in amnesia". Neuropsychologia 23 (5): 615–622. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(85)90063-6. PMID 4058707. 
  20. ^ Shimamura, A.P.; Squire, L.R. (1984). "Paired-associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: a neuropsychological approach". J. Exp. Psychol. 113: 556–570. 
  21. ^ Blaxton, T.A. (1992). "Dissociations among memory measures in memory-impaired subjects: evidence for a processing account of memory". Mem. Cogn. 15: 549–562. 
  22. ^ Graf, P.; Squire, L.R.; Mandler, G. (1984). "The information that amnesic patients do not forget". J. Exp. Psychol. 10: 164–178. 
  23. ^ Carlesimo, G.A.; Oscarberman, M. (1992). "Memory deficits in Alzheimer's patients: a comprehensive review". Neuropsychology Review 3 (2): 119–169. doi:10.1007/BF01108841. PMID 1300219. 
  24. ^ Schacter, D.L.; McGlynn, S.M.; Milberg, W.P.; Church, B.A. (1993). "Spared priming despite impaired comprehension: implicit memory in a case of word meaning deafness". Neuropsychology 7 (2): 107–118. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.7.2.107. 
  25. ^ Geva, A.; Moscovitch, M.; Leach, L. (1997). "Perceptual priming of proper names in young and older normal adults and a patient with prosopanomia". Neuropsychology 11 (2): 232–242. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.11.2.232. PMID 9110330. 
  26. ^ Li L, Miller EK, Desimone R. (1993). "The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex". J Neurophysiol 69 (6): 1918–29. PMID 8350131. 
  27. ^ Gruber, T; Müller, MM (2002). "Effects of picture repetition on induced gamma band responses, evoked potentials, and phase synchrony in the human EEG". Brain research. Cognitive brain research 13 (3): 377–92. PMID 11919002. 
  28. ^ Squire, L.R.; Ojemann, J.G.; Miezin, F.M.; Petersen, S.E.; Videen, T.O.; Raichle, M.E. (1992). "Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89 (5): 1837–1841. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.5.1837. PMC 48548. PMID 1542680. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=48548. 
  29. ^ Wig, GS; Grafton, ST; Demos, KE; Kelley, WM (2005). "Reductions in neural activity underlie behavioral components of repetition priming". Nature neuroscience 8 (9): 1228–33. doi:10.1038/nn1515. PMID 16056222. 
  30. ^ Moldakarimov S, Bazhenov M, Sejnowski TJ. (2010). "Perceptual priming leads to reduction of gamma frequency oscillations". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107 (12): 5640–5645. doi:10.1073/pnas.0907525107. PMC 2851786. PMID 20212165. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2851786. 
  31. ^ Demb, J.B.; Desmond, J.E.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Vaidya, CJ; Glover, GH; Gabrieli, JD (1995). "Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity". J. Neurosci 15 (9): 5870–5878. PMID 7666172. 
  32. ^ Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Poldrack, R.A.; Desmond, J.E. (1998). "The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory". PNAS 95 (3): 906–913. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.906. PMC 33815. PMID 9448258. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=33815. 
  33. ^ Dehaene, S; Naccache, L; Le Clec'h, G; Koechlin, E; Mueller, M; Dehaene-Lambertz, G; Van De Moortele, PF; Le Bihan, D (1998). "Imaging unconscious semantic priming". Nature 395 (6702): 597–600. doi:10.1038/26967. PMID 9783584. http://www.casra.ch/teaching/2002/cns/doc/Dehaene_et_al_1998.pdf. 
  34. ^ a b c d e f Bargh, John A.; Chen, Mark; Burrows, Laura (1996). "Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71 (2): 230–44. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230. PMID 8765481. 
  35. ^ (see Ironic process theory for a more in-depth discussion of this phenomenon)
  36. ^ IJerman, Hans; Semin, Gun R. (2009). "The Thermometer of Social Relations: Mapping Social Proximity on Temperature". Psychological Science 20 (10): 1214–1220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02434.x. PMID 19732385. 

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Priming — may refer to:* Priming (psychology), a manifestation of implicit memory * Priming , also known as Agenda setting theory, the process of bringing certain content to the foreground in the media * Priming (agriculture), a form of seed planting… …   Wikipedia

  • Priming (media) — Priming is a theory in which the activation of one thought may trigger related thoughts. The priming theory states that media images stimulate related thoughts in the minds of audience members.[1] For example, if a person were to see a cartoon… …   Wikipedia

  • Priming (Psychologie) — Das Priming als Fachbegriff der Psychologie ist die Beeinflussung der Verarbeitung (Kognition) eines Reizes dadurch, dass ein vorangegangener Reiz implizite Gedächtnisinhalte aktiviert hat. Der primende Reiz aktiviert Kontextinformationen, die… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Negative Priming — Pens. Initially, the person picks out the red pen and therefore it is the prime target while the remaining pens in the holder are considered to be prime distractors. When the person wants to use the blue pen (probe target) instead, negative… …   Wikipedia

  • Response Priming — Mit dem Begriff Response Priming bezeichnet man eine besondere Form des Priming in der Wahrnehmungspsychologie. Allgemein bestehen Priming Effekte darin, dass die Reaktion (engl. Response) auf einen Zielreiz (engl. Target) von der vorausgehenden… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Negatives Priming — Als negatives Priming wird in der Psychologie die verlangsamte Reaktion auf einen vorher ignorierten Reiz bezeichnet. Die vorherige Darbietung eines Reizes (des Primes) beeinflusst somit die Verarbeitungszeit eines nachfolgenden Zielreizes (des… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Schema (psychology) — A schema (pl. schemata ), in psychology and cognitive science, is a mental structure that represents some aspect of the world. This learning theory views organized knowledge as an elaborate network of abstract mental structures which represent… …   Wikipedia

  • Semantisches Priming — Als semantisches Priming wird in der Psychologie der Priming Effekt bezeichnet, dass die Verarbeitung eines Wortes die Verarbeitung eines zweiten nachfolgenden Wortes beeinflusst, falls zwischen beiden Wörtern eine semantische, z. B.… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Medien-Priming — bezeichnet einen Prozess, bei dem durch medial vermittelte Information Wissenseinheiten darunter auch Erinnerungen, Vorwissen, Voreinstellungen aktiviert werden, um sich später eine Meinung zu bilden oder Entscheidungen zu treffen.… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Affekt-Priming-Modell — Das Affekt Priming Modell (Affect priming model) bezeichnet ein von J.P. Forgas vorgelegtes Modell,[1] das auf Erkenntnissen der Forschung zum Priming aufbaut und das die selektive Aktivierung und erhöhte Zugänglichkeit von mit dem Affekt oder… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”