The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two

The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two

"The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information" is one of the most highly cited papers in psychology.[2][3][4] It was published in 1956 by the cognitive psychologist George A. Miller of Princeton University's Department of Psychology in Psychological Review. It supposedly argues that the number of objects an average human can hold in working memory is 7 ± 2. This is frequently referred to as Miller's Law (not to be confused with his theory of communication: Miller's Law).

Recent research has demonstrated that not only is the "law" based on a misinterpretation of Miller's paper, but that the correct number is probably around three or four.[5]

Contents

Miller's article

In his article, Miller discussed a coincidence between the limits of one-dimensional absolute judgment and the limits of short-term memory. In a one-dimensional absolute-judgment task, a person is presented with a number of stimuli that vary on one dimension (e.g., 10 different tones varying only in pitch) and responds to each stimulus with a corresponding response (learned before). Performance is nearly perfect up to 5 or 6 different stimuli but declines as the number of different stimuli is increased. The task can be described as one of information transmission: The input consists of one out of n possible stimuli, and the output consists of one out of n responses. The information contained in the input can be determined by the number of binary decisions that need to be made to arrive at the selected stimulus, and the same holds for the response. Therefore, people's maximum performance on one-dimensional absolute judgement can be characterized as an information channel capacity with approximately 2 to 3 bits of information, which corresponds to the ability to distinguish between 4 and 8 alternatives.

The second cognitive limitation Miller discusses is memory span. Memory span refers to the longest list of items (e.g., digits, letters, words) that a person can repeat back immediately after presentation in correct order on 50% of trials. Miller observed that memory span of young adults is approximately 7 items. He noticed that memory span is approximately the same for stimuli with vastly different amount of information - for instance, binary digits have 1 bit each; decimal digits have 3.32 bits each; words have about 10 bits each. Miller concluded that memory span is not limited in terms of bits but rather in terms of chunks. A chunk is the largest meaningful unit in the presented material that the person recognizes - thus, it depends on the knowledge of the person what counts as a chunk. For instance, a word is a single chunk for a speaker of the language but breaks down into as many chunks as the word has letters for someone who is totally unfamiliar with the language.

Miller recognized that the correspondence between the limits of one-dimensional absolute judgment and of short-term memory span was only a coincidence, because only the first limit, not the second, can be characterized in information-theoretic terms (i.e., as a roughly constant number of bits). Therefore, there is nothing "magical" about the number 7, and Miller used the expression only rhetorically. Nevertheless, the idea of a "magical number 7" inspired much theorizing, rigorous and less rigorous, about the capacity limits of human cognition.

The "magical number 7" and working memory capacity

Later research on short-term memory and working memory revealed that memory span is not a constant even when measured in terms of a number of chunks. The number of chunks a human can recall immediately after presentation depends on the category of chunks used (e.g., span is around seven for digits, around six for letters, and around five for words), and even on features of the chunks within a category. For instance, span is lower for long words than it is for short words. In general, memory span for verbal contents (digits, letters, words, etc.) strongly depends on the time it takes to speak the contents aloud. Some researchers have therefore proposed that the limited capacity of short-term memory for verbal material is not a "magic number" but rather a "magic spell".[6] Baddeley used this finding to postulate that one component of his model of working memory, the phonological loop, is capable of holding around 2 seconds of sound.[7][8] However, the limit of short-term memory cannot easily be characterized as a constant "magic spell" either, because memory span depends also on other factors besides speaking duration. For instance, span depends on the lexical status of the contents (i.e., whether the contents are words known to the person or not).[9] Several other factors also affect a person's measured span, and therefore it is difficult to pin down the capacity of short-term or working memory to a number of chunks. Nonetheless, Cowan (2001)[10] has proposed that working memory has a capacity of about four chunks in young adults (and less in children and older adults).[citation needed]

Tarnow (2010) finds that in a classic experiment typically argued as supporting a 4 item buffer by Murdock (1962).[citation needed] There is in fact no evidence for such and thus the "magical number", at least in the Murdock experiment, is 1.[citation needed]

Other cognitive numeric limits

Cowan also noted a number of other limits of cognition that point to a "magical number four",[10] and different from Miller, he argued that this correspondence is no coincidence. One other process that seems to be limited at about four elements is subitizing, the rapid enumeration of small numbers of objects. When a number of objects is flashed briefly, their number can be determined very quickly, at a glance, when the number does not exceed the subitizing limit, which is about four objects. Larger numbers of objects must be counted, which is a slower process. The film Rain Man portrayed an autistic savant, who was able to rapidly determine the number of toothpicks from an entire box spilled on the floor, apparently subitizing a much larger number than four objects. A similar feat was informally observed by neuropsychologist Oliver Sacks and reported in his book The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat. (Autistic expert Daniel Tammet has suggested that the children Sacks observed may have pre-counted the matches in the box.) Therefore one might suppose that this limit is an arbitrary limit imposed by our cognition rather than necessarily being a physical limit.[original research?]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Miller, George A (2003). "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (3): 141–4. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9. PMID 12639696. 
  2. ^ Gorenflo, Daniel; McConnell, James (1991). "The Most Frequently Cited Journal Articles and Authors in Introductory Psychology Textbooks". Teaching of Psychology 18: 8–12. doi:10.1207/s15328023top1801_2. 
  3. ^ Kintsch, Walter; Cacioppo, John T. (1994). "Introduction to the 100th anniversary issue of the Psychological Review". Psychological Review 101 (2): 195–9. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.2.195. http://psychology.uchicago.edu/people/faculty/cacioppo/jtcreprints/kc94.pdf. 
  4. ^ Garfield, Eugene (1985). "The Articles most cited in the SCI from 1961 to 1982. 7. Another 100 Citation Classics: The Watson-Crick Double Helix has its turn". Essays of an Information Scientist: 1985, Ghost writing and other essays. Philadelphia: ISI Press. pp. 187–96. ISBN 978-0-89495-000-1. http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/essays/v8p187y1985.pdf. 
  5. ^ Farrington, Jeanne (2011). "Seven plus or minus two". Performance Improvement Quarterly 23 (4): 113–6. doi:10.1002/piq.20099. 
  6. ^ Schweickert, Richard; Boruff, Brian (1986). "Short-term memory capacity: Magic number or magic spell?". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12 (3): 419–25. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.12.3.419. 
  7. ^ Baddeley, A (1992). "Working memory". Science 255 (5044): 556–9. doi:10.1126/science.1736359. PMID 1736359. 
  8. ^ Baddeley, Alan (2000). "The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (11): 417–23. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2. PMID 11058819. 
  9. ^ Hulme, Charles; Roodenrys, Steven; Brown, Gordon; Mercer, Robin (1995). "The role of long-term memory mechanisms in memory span". British Journal of Psychology 86 (4): 527–36. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02570.x. 
  10. ^ a b Cowan, Nelson (2001). "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1): 87–114; discussion 114–85. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922. PMID 11515286. 

References

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно сделать НИР?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two — George Armitage Miller George Armitage Miller (né le 3 février 1920 à Charleston en Virginie Occidentale aux États Unis) est professeur de psychologie à l université de Princeton depuis 1979. Ses travaux de recherche ont contribué à fonder la… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Магическое число семь (magical number seven) — Словосочетание М.ч.с. стало частью языка психологии благодаря важной статье под названием «Магическое число семь, плюс минус два: некоторые пределы нашей способности обрабатывать информацию» (The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some… …   Психологическая энциклопедия

  • 7 (number) — Seven redirects here. For other uses, see Seven (disambiguation). 7 −1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 → List of numbers Integers …   Wikipedia

  • Le nombre magique sept, plus ou moins deux — Le nombre magique sept, plus ou moins deux : quelques limites à nos capacités de traitement de l information[note 1] (Miller, 1956) est un des articles les plus cités en psychologie[1],[2],[3] …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Memory for the future — refers to the ability to use memory to picture and plan future events. It is a subcategory of mental time travel which Suddendorf and Corballis described to be the process that allows people to imagine both past and potential future events.… …   Wikipedia

  • Magic number — may refer to: Magic number (chemistry), number of atoms or molecules forming an exceptionally stable cluster Magic number (sports), a number that indicates how close a team is to winning a season title Magic number (oil), the price per barrel of… …   Wikipedia

  • Cognitive revolution — The cognitive revolution is the name for an intellectual movement in the 1950s that began what are known collectively as the cognitive sciences. It began in the modern context of greater interdisciplinary communication and research. The relevant… …   Wikipedia

  • information theory — the mathematical theory concerned with the content, transmission, storage, and retrieval of information, usually in the form of messages or data, and esp. by means of computers. [1945 50] * * * ▪ mathematics Introduction       a mathematical… …   Universalium

  • Methods used to study memory — The study of memory incorporates research methodologies from neuropsychology, human development and dragon testing using a wide range of species. The complex phenomenon of memory is explored by combining evidence from many areas of research. New… …   Wikipedia

  • Cognitive interview — The cognitive interview (CI) is a method of interviewing in which eyewitnesses and victims report what they remember from a crime scene. Using four retrievals, the primary focus of the cognitive interview is to make witnesses and victims of a… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”