Numerical cognition

Numerical cognition
Psychology
Human brain NIH.jpg
Cognitive psychology
Perception
Visual perception
Object recognition
Face recognition
Pattern recognition
Attention
Attention
Memory
Aging and memory
Emotional memory
Learning
Long-term memory
Language
Language
Thinking
Concepts
Reasoning
Decision making
Problem solving

Numerical cognition is a subdiscipline of cognitive science that studies the cognitive, developmental and neural bases of numbers and mathematics. As with many cognitive science endeavors, this is a highly interdisciplinary topic, and includes researchers in cognitive psychology, developmental psychology, neuroscience and cognitive linguistics. This discipline, although it may interact with questions in the philosophy of mathematics is primarily concerned with empirical questions.

Topics included in the domain of numerical cognition include:

  • How do non-human animals process numerosity?
  • How do infants acquire an understanding of numbers (and how much is inborn)?
  • How do humans associate linguistic symbols with numerical quantities?
  • How do these capacities underlie our ability to perform complex calculations?
  • What are the neural bases of these abilities, both in humans and in non-humans?
  • What metaphorical capacities and processes allow us to extend our numerical understanding into complex domains such as the concept of infinity, the infinitesimal or the concept of the limit in calculus?

Contents

Comparative studies

A variety of research has demonstrated that non-human animals, including rats, lions and various species of primates have an approximate sense of number (referred to as "numerosity") (for a review, see Dehaene 1997). For example, when a rat is trained to press a bar 8 or 16 times to receive a food reward, the number of bar presses will approximate a Gaussian or Normal distribution with peak around 8 or 16 bar presses. When rats are more hungry, their bar pressing behavior is more rapid, so by showing that the peak number of bar presses is the same for either well-fed or hungry rats, it is possible to disentangle time and number of bar presses.

Similarly, researchers have set up hidden speakers in the African jungle to test natural (untrained) behavior in lions (McComb, Packer & Pusey 1994). These speakers can play a number of lion calls, from 1 to 5. If a single lionness hears, for example, three calls from unknown lions, she will leave, while if she is with four of her sisters, they will go and explore. This suggests that not only can lions tell when they are "outnumbered" but that they can do this on the basis of signals from different sensory modalities, suggesting that numerosity is a multisensory concept.

Developmental studies

Developmental psychology studies have shown that human infants, like non-human animals, have an approximate sense of number. For example, in one study, infants were repeatedly presented with arrays of (in one block) 16 dots. Careful controls were in place to eliminate information from "non-numerical" parameters such as total surface area, luminance, circumference, and so on. After the infants had been presented with many displays containing 16 items, they habituated, or stopped looking as long at the display. Infants were then presented with a display containing 8 items, and they looked longer at the novel display.

Because of the numerous controls that were in place to rule out non-numerical factors, the experimenters infer that six month-old infants are sensitive to differences between 8 and 16. Subsequent experiments, using similar methodologies showed that 6 month old infants can discriminate numbers differing by a 2:1 ratio (8 vs. 16 or 16 vs. 32) but not by a 3:2 ratio (8 vs. 12 or 16 vs. 24). However, 10 month old infants succeed both at the 2:1 and the 3:2 ratio, suggesting an increased sensitivity to numerosity differences with age (for a review of this literature see Feigenson, Dehaene & Spelke 2004).

In another series of studies, Karen Wynn and her colleagues showed that infants as young as four months are able to do very simple additions (e.g., 1 + 1 = 2) and subtractions (3 - 1 = 2). To demonstrate this, Wynn and colleagues used a "violation of expectation" paradigm, in which infants were shown (for example) one Mickey Mouse doll going behind a screen, followed by another. If, when the screen was lowered, infants were presented with only one Mickey (the "impossible event") they looked longer than if they were shown two Mickeys (the "possible" event). However, this is only true if the number of items does not exceed four (e.g., infants fail at 5 - 2 = 3).

There is debate about how much these infant systems actually contain in terms of number concepts, harkening to the classic nature versus nurture debate. Gelman and Gallistel (1978) suggested that a child innately has the concept of natural number, and only has to map this onto the words used in her language. Susan Carey (2004, 2009) disagreed, saying that these systems can only encoded large numbers in an approximate way, where language-based natural numbers can be exact. One promising approach is to see if cultures that lack number words can deal with natural numbers. The results so far are mixed (e.g., Pica, Lemer, Izard & Dehaene, 2004; Butterworth, Reevet, Reynolds & Lloyd, 2008).

Neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies

Human neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that regions of the parietal lobe, including the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) are activated when subjects are asked to perform calculation tasks. Based on both human neuroimaging and neuropsychology, Stanislas Dehaene and colleagues have suggested that these two parietal structures play complementary roles. The IPS is thought to house the circuitry that is fundamentally involved in numerical estimation (Piazza et al. 2004), number comparison (Pinel et al. 2001; Pinel et al. 2004) and on-line calculation (often tested with subtraction) while the IPL is thought to be involved in overlearned tasks, such as multiplication (see Dehaene 1997). Thus, a patient with a lesion to the IPL may be able to subtract, but not multiply, and vice versa for a patient with a lesion to the IPS. In addition to these parietal regions, regions of the frontal lobe are also active in calculation tasks. These activations overlap with regions involved in language processing such as Broca's area and regions involved in working memory and attention. Future research will be needed to disentangle the complex influences of language, working memory and attention on numerical processes.

Single-unit neurophysiology in monkeys has also found neurons in the frontal cortex and in the intraparietal sulcus that respond to numbers. Andreas Nieder (Nieder 2005; Nieder, Freedman & Miller 2002; Nieder & Miller 2004) trained monkeys to perform a "delayed match-to-sample" task. For example, a monkey might be presented with a field of four dots, and is required to keep that in memory after the display is taken away. Then, after a delay period of several seconds, a second display is presented. If the number on the second display match that from the first, the monkey has to release a lever. If it is different, the monkey has to hold the lever. Neural activity recorded during the delay period showed that neurons in the intraparietal sulcus and the frontal cortex had a "preferred numerosity", exactly as predicted by behavioral studies. That is, a certain number might fire strongly for four, but less strongly for three or five, and even less for two or six. Thus, we say that these neurons were "tuned" for specific numerosities. Note that these neuronal responses followed Weber's law, as has been demonstrated for other sensory dimensions, and consistent with the ratio dependence observed for non-human animals' and infants' numerical behavior (Nieder & Miller 2003).

Relations between number and other cognitive processes

There is evidence that numerical cognition is intimately related to other aspects of thought – particularly spatial cognition.[1] One line of evidence comes from studies performed on number-form synaesthetes.[2] Such individuals report that numbers are mentally represented with a particular spatial layout; others experience numbers as perceivable objects that can be visually manipulated to facilitate calculation. Behavioral studies further reinforce the connection between numerical and spatial cognition. For instance, participants respond quicker to larger numbers if they are responding on the right side of space, and quicker to smaller numbers when on the left—the so-called SNARC effect.[3] This effect varies across culture and context,[4] however, and some research has even begun to question whether the SNARC reflects an inherent number-space association,[5] instead invoking strategic problem solving or a more general cognitive mechanism like conceptual metaphor.[6][7] Moreover, neuroimaging studies reveal that the association between number and space also shows up in brain activity. Regions of the parietal cortex, for instance, show shared activation for both spatial and numerical processing.[8] These various lines of research suggest a strong, but flexible, connection between numerical and spatial cognition.

Works cited

  1. ^ Hubbard, Edward M.; Piazza, Manuela; Pinel, Philippe; Dehaene, Stanislas (June 2005). "Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6 (1-2): 435–448. doi:10.1038/nrn1684. PMID 15928716. http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v6/n6/full/nrn1684.html. 
  2. ^ Galton, Francis (25). "Visualised Numerals". Nature 21 (543): 494–495. doi:10.1038/021494e0. http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v21/n543/abs/021494e0.html. 
  3. ^ Dehaene, Stanislas; Bossini, Serge; Giraux, Pascal (September 1993). "The mental representation of parity and number magnitude". Journal of Experimental Psychology 122 (3): 371–396. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371. http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/xge/122/3/371/. 
  4. ^ Fischer, Martin H.; Mills, Richard A.; Shaki, Samuel (April 2010). "How to cook a SNARC: Number placement in text rapidly changes spatial–numerical associations". Brain and Cognition 72 (3): 333–336. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.010. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6WBY-4XPBSNT-1&_user=4429&_coverDate=04%2F30%2F2010&_rdoc=1&_fmt=high&_orig=gateway&_origin=gateway&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_searchStrId=1750488780&_rerunOrigin=scholar.google&_acct=C000059602&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=4429&md5=2c807ea3dafb118b016973bfee5441f6&searchtype=a. Retrieved 14 November 2009. 
  5. ^ Núñez, Rafael; Doan, D., Nikoulina, A. (in press). "Squeezing, striking, and vocalizing: Is number representation fundamentally spatial?". Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.001. PMID 21640338. 
  6. ^ Walsh, Vincent (November 2003). "A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (11): 483–488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6VH9-49JHRY2-1&_user=9044837&_coverDate=11%2F30%2F2003&_rdoc=1&_fmt=high&_orig=gateway&_origin=gateway&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_acct=C000059602&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=9044837&md5=8621b8ecd2a24915cdcf9294cda7783c&searchtype=a. 
  7. ^ Nunez, Rafael (2009). "Numbers and Arithmetic: Neither Hardwired Nor Out There". Biological Theory 4(1): 68–83. doi:10.1162/biot.2009.4.1.68. http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/biot.2009.4.1.68. Retrieved 29 December 2009. 
  8. ^ Dehaene, Stanislas (1992). "Varieties of numerical abilities". Cognition 44 (1-2): 1–42. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6T24-45Y7TH4-Y&_user=4429&_coverDate=12%2F31%2F1992&_rdoc=1&_fmt=high&_orig=gateway&_origin=gateway&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_searchStrId=1750188945&_rerunOrigin=scholar.google&_acct=C000059602&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=4429&md5=49a1400aa8264679fcd71fd934c67996&searchtype=a#implicit0. Retrieved 30 May 2002. 

See also

References

  • Dehaene, S. (1997), The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics, New York: Oxford University Press, ISBN 0195132408, http://www.unicog.org 
  • Feigenson, L.; Dehaene, S.; Spelke, E. (2004), "Core systems of number", Trends in Cognitive Science 8 (7): 307–314 
  • Lakoff, G.; Nuñez, R. E. (2000), Where mathematics comes from, New York: Basic Books., ISBN 0465037704 
  • McComb, K.; Packer, C.; Pusey, A. (1994), "Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo", Animal Behavior 47: 379–387 
  • Nieder, A. (2005), "Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence", Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 177–190 
  • Nieder, A.; Freedman, D. J.; Miller, E. K. (2002), "Representation of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex", Science 297: 1708–1711 
  • Nieder, A.; Miller, E. K. (2003), "Coding of cognitive magnitude: Compressed scaling of numerical information in the primate prefrontal cortex", Neuron 37: 149–157 
  • Nieder, A.; Miller, E. K. (2004), "A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in the monkey", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 7457–7462 
  • Piazza, M.; Izard, V.; Pinel, P.; Le Bihan, D.; Dehaene, S. (2004), "Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus", Neuron 44: 547–555 
  • Pinel, P.; Dehaene, S.; Riviere, D.; Le Bihan, D. (2001), "Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task", Neuroimage 14 (5): 1013–1026 
  • Pinel, P.; Piazza, M.; Le Bihan, D.; Dehaene, S. (2004), "Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments", Neuron 41 (6): 983–993 

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Cognition — In science, cognition refers to mental processes. These processes include attention, remembering, producing and understanding language, solving problems, and making decisions. Cognition is studied in various disciplines such as psychology,… …   Wikipedia

  • Animal cognition — is the title given to the study of the mental capacities of non human animals. It has developed out of comparative psychology, but has also been strongly influenced by the approach of ethology, behavioral ecology, and evolutionary psychology. The …   Wikipedia

  • Stanislas Dehaene — Infobox Scientist image width = 150px name = Stanislas Dehaene birth date = May 12, 1965 birth place = Roubaix, France death date = death place = residence = flagicon|France Palaiseau, France nationality = flagicon|France French field = Cognitive …   Wikipedia

  • Educational neuroscience — (also called Mind Brain and Education; MBE) is an emerging scientific field that brings together researchers in cognitive neuroscience, developmental cognitive neuroscience, educational psychology, educational technology, education theory and… …   Wikipedia

  • Synesthesia — For other uses, see Synesthesia (disambiguation). How someone with synesthesia might perceive certain letters and numbers. Synesthesia (also spelled synæsthesia or synaesthesia, plural synesthesiae or synaesthesiae), from the ancient Greek σύν… …   Wikipedia

  • Numerosity adaptation effect — Psychology Cognitive psychology Perception …   Wikipedia

  • Parity of zero — Zero objects, divided into two equal groups Zero is an even number. In other words, its parity the quality of an integer being even or odd is even. Zero fits the definition of even number : it is an integer multiple of 2, namely 0 × 2. As a… …   Wikipedia

  • Number form — This article refers to the neurological phenomenon. For Unicode numbers, see Number Forms. A number form from one of Francis Galton s (1881b) subjects. Note the convolutions, and how the first 12 digits correspond to a clock face. A number form… …   Wikipedia

  • Psychological pricing — Example of psychological pricing at a gas station Psychological pricing or price ending is a marketing practice based on the theory that certain prices have a psychological impact. The retail prices are often expressed as odd prices : a little… …   Wikipedia

  • Dyscalculia — Classification and external resources Lateral surface of left cerebral hemisphere, viewed from the side. (Intraparietal sulcus visible at upper right, running horizontally.) ICD …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”