Problem solving

Problem solving

Problem solving forms part of thinking. Considered the most complex of all intellectual functions, problem solving has been defined as higher-order cognitive process that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills (Goldstein & Levin, 1987). It occurs if an organism or an artificial intelligence system does not know how to proceed from a given state to a desired goal state. It is part of the larger problem process that includes problem finding and problem shaping.

Overview

The nature of human problem solving methods has been studied by psychologists over the past hundred years. There are several methods of studying problem solving, including; introspection, behaviorism, simulation and computer modeling, and experiment.

Beginning with the early experimental work of the Gestaltists in Germany (e.g. Duncker, 1935), and continuing through the 1960s and early 1970s, research on problem solving typically conducted relatively simple, laboratory tasks (e.g. Duncker's "X-ray" problem; Ewert & Lambert's 1932 "disk" problem, later known as Tower of Hanoi) that appeared novel to participants (e.g. Mayer, 1992). Various reasons account for the choice of simple novel tasks: they had clearly defined optimal solutions, they were solvable within a relatively short time frame, researchers could trace participants' problem-solving steps, and so on. The researchers made the underlying assumption, of course, that simple tasks such as the Tower of Hanoi captured the main properties of "real world" problems, and that the cognitive processes underlying participants' attempts to solve simple problems were representative of the processes engaged in when solving "real world" problems. Thus researchers used simple problems for reasons of convenience, and thought generalizations to more complex problems would become possible. Perhaps the best-known and most impressive example of this line of research remains the work by Newell and Simon (1972).

USA and Canada

In North America, initiated by the work of Herbert Simon on learning by doing in semantically rich domains (e.g. Anzai & Simon, 1979; Bhaskar & Simon, 1977), researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural knowledge domains - such as physics, writing, or chess playing - thus relinquishing their attempts to extract a global theory of problem solving (e.g. Sternberg & Frensch, 1991). Instead, these researchers have frequently focused on the development of problem solving within a certain domain, that is on the development of expertise (e.g. Anderson, Boyle & Reiser, 1985; Chase & Simon, 1973; Chi, Feltovich & Glaser, 1981).

Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include such diverse fields as:
*Reading (Stanovich & Cunningham, 1991)
*Writing (Bryson, Bereiter, Scardamalia & Joram, 1991)
*Calculation (Sokol & McCloskey, 1991)
*Political decision making (Voss, Wolfe, Lawrence & Engle, 1991)
*Managerial problem solving (Wagner, 1991)
*Lawyers' reasoning (Amsel, Langer & Loutzenhiser, 1991)
*Mechanical problem solving (Hegarty, 1991)
*Problem solving in electronics (Lesgold & Lajoie, 1991)
*Computer skills (Kay, 1991)
*Game playing (Frensch & Sternberg, 1991)
*Personal problem solving (Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987)
*Mathematical problem solving (Polya, 1945; Schoenfeld, 1985)
*Social problem solving (D'Zurilla & Goldfreid, 1971; D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1982)

Europe

In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by Donald Broadbent (1977; see Berry & Broadbent, 1995) in the United Kingdom and the other one by Dietrich Dörner (1975, 1985; see Dörner & Wearing, 1995) in Germany. The two approaches have in common an emphasis on relatively complex, semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and methodology, however. The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem-solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and typically employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition initiated by Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive, motivational, and social components of problem solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up to 2,000 highly interconnected variables (e.g., Dörner, Kreuzig, Reither & Stäudel's 1983 LOHHAUSEN project; Ringelband, Misiak & Kluwe, 1990). Buchner (1995) describes the two traditions in detail.

To sum up, researchers' realization that problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise (e.g. Sternberg, 1995) and that, consequently, findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to problem-solving situations outside the laboratory, has during the past two decades led to an emphasis on real-world problem solving. This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge domains, much of the European research has focused on novel, complex problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios (see Funke, 1991, for an overview).

Characteristics of difficult problems

As elucidated by Dietrich Dörner and later expanded upon by Joachim Funke, difficult problems have some typical characteristics that can be summarized as follows:

*Intransparency (lack of clarity of the situation)
**commencement opacity
**continuation opacity
*Polytely (multiple goals)
**inexpressiveness
**opposition
**transience
*Complexity (large numbers of items, interrelations, and decisions)
**enumerability
**connectivity (hierarchy relation, communication relation, allocation relation)
**heterogeneity
*Dynamics (time considerations)
**temporal constraints
**temporal sensitivity
**phase effects
**dynamic unpredictability

The resolution of difficult problems requires a direct attack on each of these characteristics that are encountered.

In reform mathematics, greater emphasis is placed on problem solving relative to basic skills, where basic operations can be done with calculators. However some "problems" may actually have standard solutions taught in higher grades. For example, kindergarteners could be asked how many fingers are there on all the gloves of 3 children, which can be solved with multiplication. [2007 Draft, Washington State Revised Mathematics Standard]

ome problem-solving techniques

There are many approaches to problem solving, depending on the nature of the problem and the people involved in the problem. The more traditional, rational approach is typically used and involves, e.g., clarifying description of the problem, analyzing causes, identifying alternatives, assessing each alternative, choosing one, implementing it, and evaluating whether the problem was solved or not.

Another, more state-of-the-art approach is appreciative inquiry. That approach asserts that "problems" are often the result of our own perspectives on a phenomenon, e.g., if we look at it as a "problem," then it will become one and we'll probably get very stuck on the "problem." Appreciative inquiry includes identification of our best times about the situation in the past, wishing and thinking about what worked best then, visioning what we want in the future, and building from our strengths to work toward our vision. [ [http://www.managementhelp.org/prsn_prd/prob_slv.htm Problem Solving ] ]

# divide and conquer: break down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems.
# Hill-climbing strategy, (or - rephrased - gradient descent/ascent, difference reduction) - attempting at every step to move closer to the goal situation. The problem with this approach is that many challenges require that you seem to move away from the goal state in order to clearly see the solution.
# Means-end analysis, more effective than hill-climbing, requires the setting of subgoals based on the process of getting from the initial state to the goal state when solving a problem.
# Trial-and-error (also called guess and check)
# Brainstorming
# Morphological analysis
# Method of focal objects
# Lateral thinking
# George Pólya's techniques in "How to Solve It"
# Research: study what others have written about the problem (and related problems). Maybe there's already a solution?
# Assumption reversal (write down your assumptions about the problem, and then reverse them all)
# Analogy: has a similar problem (possibly in a different field) been solved before?
# Hypothesis testing: assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove the assumption.
# Constraint examination: are you assuming a constraint which doesn't really exist?
# Incubation: input the details of a problem into your mind, then stop focusing on it. The subconscious mind will continue to work on the problem, and the solution might just "pop up" while you are doing something else
# Build (or write) one or more abstract models of the problem
# Try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. Where the proof breaks down can be your starting point for resolving it
# Get help from friends or online problem solving community (e.g. 3form, InnoCentive)
# delegation: delegating the problem to others.
# Root Cause Analysis
# Working Backwards (Halpern,2002)
# Forward-Looking Strategy (Halpern, 2002)
# Simplification (Halpern, 2002)
# Generalization (Halpern, 2002)
# Specialization (Halpern, 2002)
# Random Search (Halpern, 2002)
# Split-Half Method (Halpern,2002)
# [http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/GROW_model The GROW model]

ee also


* Abductive reasoning

* Analogy
* Artificial intelligence
* Brainstorming
* Common sense
* Common sense reasoning
* Creative problem solving
* Cyc
* Deductive reasoning
* Divergent thinking
* Educational psychology
* Executive function
* Facilitation
* General problem solver
* Inductive reasoning
* Innovation
* Intelligence amplification
* Inquiry
* Kepner-Tregoe
* Morphological Analysis
* Newell, Allen
* PDCA
*
* Problem Statement
* RPR Problem Diagnosis
* Simon, Herbert
* Soar (cognitive architecture)
* Thought
* Transdisciplinary Studies
* TRIZ
* Troubleshooting
* Wicked problem

Notes

References


*wikicite | id= Amsel| reference= Amsel, E., Langer, R., & Loutzenhiser, L. (1991). Do lawyers reason differently from psychologists? A comparative design for studying expertise. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 223-250). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0-8058-1783-6
*wikicite | id= Anderson| reference= cite journal
author = Anderson, J. R., Boyle, C. B., & Reiser, B. J.
title = Intelligent tutoring systems
journal = Science
date = 1985
volume = 228
pages = 456–462
doi = 10.1126/science.228.4698.456
pmid = 17746875

*wikicite | id= Anzai| reference= cite journal
author=Anzai, K., & Simon, H. A. (1979)
title=The theory of learning by doing
journal=Psychological Review
volume=86
pages=124–140
url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=493441&dopt=Abstract
doi=10.1037/0033-295X.86.2.124
year=1979

*wikicite | id= Beckmann| reference= Beckmann, J. F., & Guthke, J. (1995). Complex problem solving, intelligence, and learning ability. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 177-200). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Berry| reference= Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1995). Implicit learning in the control of complex systems: A reconsideration of some of the earlier claims. In P.A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 131-150). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Bhaskar| reference= Bhaskar, R., & Simon, H. A. (1977). Problem solving in semantically rich domains: An example from engineering thermodynamics. "Cognitive Science", 1, 193-215.
*wikicite | id= Brehmer1995| reference= Brehmer, B. (1995). Feedback delays in dynamic decision making. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 103-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Brehmer1993| reference= Brehmer, B., & Dörner, D. (1993). Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study. "Computers in Human Behavior", 9, 171-184.
*wikicite | id= Broadbent| reference= Broadbent, D. E. (1977). Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. "Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology", 29, 181-201.
*wikicite | id= Bryson| reference= Bryson, M., Bereiter, C., Scardamalia, M., & Joram, E. (1991). Going beyond the problem as given: Problem solving in expert and novice writers. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 61-84). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Buchner1995a| reference= Buchner, A. (1995). Theories of complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 27-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Buchner1995b| reference= Buchner, A., Funke, J., & Berry, D. C. (1995). Negative correlations between control performance and verbalizable knowledge: Indicators for implicit learning in process control tasks? "Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology", 48A, 166-187.
*wikicite | id= Chase| reference= Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. "Cognitive Psychology", 4, 55-81.
*wikicite | id= Chi| reference= cite journal
author=Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R.
year=1981
title=Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices
journal=Cognitive Science
volume=5
pages=121–152
url=http://www.usabilityviews.com/uv007206.html

*wikicite | id= Dörner1975| reference= Dörner, D. (1975). Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten [How people wanted to improve the world] . "Bild der Wissenschaft", 12, 48-53.
*wikicite | id= Dörner1985| reference= Dörner, D. (1985). Verhalten, Denken und Emotionen [Behavior, thinking, and emotions] . In L. H. Eckensberger & E. D. Lantermann (Eds.), "Emotion und Reflexivität" (pp. 157-181). München, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg.
*wikicite | id= Dörner1992| reference= Dörner, D. (1992). Über die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder "Computerszenarios" in der psychologischen Forschung [On the proper use of microworlds or "computer scenarios" in psychological research] . In H. Gundlach (Ed.), "Psychologische Forschung und Methode: Das Versprechen des Experiments. Festschrift für Werner Traxel" (pp. 53-87). Passau, Germany: Passavia-Universitäts-Verlag.
*wikicite | id= Dörner1983| reference= Dörner, D., Kreuzig, H. W., Reither, F., & Stäudel, T. (Eds.). (1983). "Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität" [Lohhausen. On dealing with uncertainty and complexity] . Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
*wikicite | id= Dörner1995| reference= Dörner, D., & Wearing, A. (1995). Complex problem solving: Toward a (computer-simulated) theory. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 65-99). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Duncker| reference= Duncker, K. (1935). "Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens" [The psychology of productive thinking] . Berlin: Julius Springer.
*wikicite | id= Ewert| reference= Ewert, P. H., & Lambert, J. F. (1932). Part II: The effect of verbal instructions upon the formation of a concept. "Journal of General Psychology", 6, 400-411.
*wikicite | id= Eyferth| reference= Eyferth, K., Schömann, M., & Widowski, D. (1986). Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexität [On how psychologists deal with complexity] . "Sprache & Kognition", 5, 11-26.
*wikicite | id= Frensch1995| reference= Frensch, P. A., & Funke, J. (Eds.). (1995). "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective". Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Frensch1991| reference= Frensch, P. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Skill-related differences in game playing. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 343-381). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Funke1991| reference= Funke, J. (1991). Solving complex problems: Human identification and control of complex systems. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 185-222). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Funke1993| reference= Funke, J. (1993). Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results. In G. Strube & K.-F. Wender (Eds.), "The cognitive psychology of knowledge" (pp. 313-330). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
*wikicite | id= Funke1995a| reference= Funke, J. (1995). Experimental research on complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 243-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Funke1995b| reference= Funke, U. (1995). Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 219-240). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Goldstein | reference= Goldstein F. C., & Levin H. S. (1987). Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability. In M. Meier, A. Benton, & L. Diller (Eds.), "Neuropsychological rehabilitation". London: Taylor & Francis Group.

*wikicite | id= Groner| reference= Groner, M., Groner, R., & Bischof, W. F. (1983). Approaches to heuristics: A historical review. In R. Groner, M. Groner, & W. F. Bischof (Eds.), "Methods of heuristics" (pp. 1-18). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id=Halpern| reference= Halpern, Diane F. (2002).Thought & Knowledge. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [http://worldcat.org/oclc/50065032&tab=holdings Worldcat Library Catalog]
*wikicite | id= Hayes| reference= Hayes, J. (1980). "The complete problem solver". Philadelphia: The Franklin Institute Press.
*wikicite | id= Hegarty| reference= Hegarty, M. (1991). Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 253-285). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Heppner| reference= Heppner, P. P., & Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). An information-processing approach to personal problem solving. "The Counseling Psychologist", 15, 371-447.
*wikicite | id= Huber| reference= Huber, O. (1995). Complex problem solving as multi stage decision making. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 151-173). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Hübner| reference= Hübner, R. (1989). Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven Steuerung dynamischer Systeme [Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks] . "Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie", 36, 221-238.
*wikicite | id= Hunt| reference= Hunt, E. (1991). Some comments on the study of complexity. In R. J. Sternberg, & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 383-395). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Hussy| reference= Hussy, W. (1985). Komplexes Problemlösen - Eine Sackgasse? [Complex problem solving - a dead end?] . "Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie", 32, 55-77.
*wikicite | id= Kay| reference= Kay, D. S. (1991). Computer interaction: Debugging the problems. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 317-340). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Kluwe1993| reference= Kluwe, R. H. (1993). Knowledge and performance in complex problem solving. In G. Strube & K.-F. Wender (Eds.), "The cognitive psychology of knowledge" (pp. 401-423). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
*wikicite | id= Kluwe1995| reference= Kluwe, R. H. (1995). Single case studies and models of complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 269-291). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Kolb| reference= Kolb, S., Petzing, F., & Stumpf, S. (1992). Komplexes Problemlösen: Bestimmung der Problemlösegüte von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research ? ein interdisziplinärer Ansatz [Complex problem solving: determining the quality of human problem solving by operations research tools - an interdisciplinary approach] . "Sprache & Kognition", 11, 115-128.
*wikicite | id= Krems| reference= Krems, J. F. (1995). Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 201-218). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Lesgold| reference= Lesgold, A., & Lajoie, S. (1991). Complex problem solving in electronics. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 287-316). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Mayer| reference= Mayer, R. E. (1992). "Thinking, problem solving, cognition". Second edition. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
*wikicite | id= Müller| reference= Müller, H. (1993). "Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen" [Complex problem solving: Reliability and knowledge] . Bonn, Germany: Holos.
*wikicite | id= Newell| reference= Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). "Human problem solving". Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
*wikicite | id= Paradies| reference= Paradies, M.W., & Unger, L. W. (2000). "TapRooT - The System for Root Cause Analysis, Problem Investigation, and Proactive Improvement". Knoxville, TN: System Improvements.
*wikicite | id= Putz| reference= Putz-Osterloh, W. (1993). Strategies for knowledge acquisition and transfer of knowledge in dynamic tasks. In G. Strube & K.-F. Wender (Eds.), "The cognitive psychology of knowledge" (pp. 331-350). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
*wikicite | id= Riefer| reference= Riefer, D.M., & Batchelder, W.H. (1988). Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes. "Psychological Review", 95, 318-339.
*wikicite | id= Ringelband| reference= Ringelband, O. J., Misiak, C., & Kluwe, R. H. (1990). Mental models and strategies in the control of a complex system. In D. Ackermann, & M. J. Tauber (Eds.), "Mental models and human-computer interaction" (Vol. 1, pp. 151-164). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
*wikicite | id= Schaub| reference= Schaub, H. (1993). "Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation". Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
*wikicite | id= Sokol| reference= Sokol, S. M., & McCloskey, M. (1991). Cognitive mechanisms in calculation. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 85-116). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Stanovich| reference= Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1991). Reading as constrained reasoning. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 3-60). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Sternberg1995| reference= Sternberg, R. J. (1995). Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison of alternative conceptions. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: The European Perspective" (pp. 295-321). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Sternberg1991| reference= Sternberg, R. J., & Frensch, P. A. (Eds.). (1991). "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms". Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Strauß| reference= Strauß, B. (1993). "Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlösen. Zum Einfluß des Anteils der richtigen Lösungen (ArL) auf das Problemlöseverhalten in komplexen Situationen" [Confoundations in complex problem solving. On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in complex situations] . Bonn, Germany: Holos.
*wikicite | id= Strohschneider| reference= Strohschneider, S. (1991). Kein System von Systemen! Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz "Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen" von Joachim Funke [No system of systems! Reply to the paper "System features as determinants of behavior in dynamic task environments" by Joachim Funke] . "Sprache & Kognition", 10, 109-113.
*wikicite | id= Van | reference= Van Lehn, K. (1989). Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), "Foundations of cognitive science" (pp. 527-579). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
*wikicite | id= Voss| reference= Voss, J. F., Wolfe, C. R., Lawrence, J. A., & Engle, R. A. (1991). From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 119-158). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Wagner| reference= Wagner, R. K. (1991). Managerial problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), "Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms" (pp. 159-183). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*wikicite | id= Wisconsin | reference= Wisconsin Educational Media Association. (1993). "Information literacy: A position paper on information problem-solving." Madison, WI: WEMA Publications. (ED 376 817). (Portions adapted from Michigan State Board of Education's Position Paper on Information Processing Skills, 1992).

External links

* [http://www.ericdigests.org/1996-4/skills.htm Computer Skills for Information Problem-Solving: Learning and Teaching Technology in Context]
* [http://moodle.ed.uiuc.edu/wiked/index.php/Problem_solving-Elementary_level Problem solving-Elementary level]
* [http://ceap.wcu.edu/houghton/Learner/basicidea.html CROP (Communities Resolving Our Problems)]
* [http://www.teach-kids-math-by-model-method.com Teach Kids Math With Model Method]


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать реферат

Look at other dictionaries:

  • problem-solving — UK US noun [uncountable] the process of finding solutions to problems Thesaurus: ways of solving problemssynonym * * * noun [noncount] : the process or act of finding a solution to a problem Let s do some problem solving and see if we can t… …   Useful english dictionary

  • problem-solving — ˈproblem ˌsolving noun [uncountable] the activity of finding answers to problems: • The course aims to help you improve your business problem solving skills. * * * problem solving UK US noun [U] HR, MANAGEMENT ► the process of finding solutions… …   Financial and business terms

  • problem-solving — problem .solving n [U] when you find ways of doing things, or answers to problems ▪ tasks that involve problem solving ▪ employees with good problem solving skills …   Dictionary of contemporary English

  • problem-solving — problem ,solving noun uncount the process of finding solutions to problems …   Usage of the words and phrases in modern English

  • problem solving — Process involved in finding a solution to a problem. Many animals routinely solve problems of locomotion, food finding, and shelter through trial and error. Some higher animals, such as apes and cetaceans, have demonstrated more complex problem… …   Universalium

  • problem-solving — noun Problem solving is used before these nouns: ↑approach, ↑skill, ↑technique …   Collocations dictionary

  • problem-solving — noun (U) finding ways of doing things, or finding answers to problems: Involve the class in a problem solving activity …   Longman dictionary of contemporary English

  • problem solving — sprendimas statusas T sritis švietimas apibrėžtis Mąstymo procesas, kai ieškoma atsakymo į teorinį ar praktinį uždavinį, problemą. Esama skirtumų tarp uždavinio ir problemos sprendimo, nes jų vidinė sandara kitokia. Sprendimo procesą sudaro šie… …   Enciklopedinis edukologijos žodynas

  • problem solving — sprendimas statusas T sritis Kūno kultūra ir sportas apibrėžtis Mąstymo vyksmas, kuriuo ieškoma atsako į teorinį ar praktinį uždavinį, problemą. atitikmenys: angl. decision; problem solving vok. Beschluss, m; Entscheidung, f; Urteil, n rus.… …   Sporto terminų žodynas

  • problem solving — sprendimas statusas T sritis Kūno kultūra ir sportas apibrėžtis Sportinės veiklos aplinkybes, sportininko elgsenos normas, užsibrėžtą tikslą labiausiai atitinkančių veiksmų pasirinkimas. Sprendimas yra tarpinis veiksmas tarp mąstymo ir judėjimo… …   Sporto terminų žodynas

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”