- Spurious relationship
-
In statistics, a spurious relationship (or, sometimes, spurious correlation or spurious regression) is a mathematical relationship in which two events or variables have no direct causal connection, yet it may be wrongly inferred that they do, due to either coincidence or the presence of a certain third, unseen factor (referred to as a "confounding factor" or "lurking variable"). Suppose there is found to be a correlation between A and B. Aside from coincidence, there are three possible relationships:
- A causes B,
- B causes A,
- OR
- C causes both A and B.
In the last case there is a spurious correlation between A and B. In a regression model where A is regressed on B but C is actually the true causal factor for A, this misleading choice of independent variable (B instead of C) is called specification error.
Because correlation can arise from the presence of a lurking variable rather than from direct causation, it is often said that "Correlation does not imply causation".
Contents
General example
An example of a spurious relationship can be illuminated examining a city's ice cream sales. These sales are highest when the rate of drownings in city swimming pools is highest. To allege that ice cream sales cause drowning, or vice-versa, would be to imply a spurious relationship between the two. In reality, a heat wave may have caused both. The heat wave is an example of a hidden or unseen variable, also known as a confounding variable.
Another popular example is a series of Dutch statistics showing a positive correlation between the number of storks nesting in a series of springs and the number of human babies born at that time. Of course there was no causal connection; they were correlated with each other only because they were correlated with the weather nine months before the observations.[1]
Detecting spurious relationships
The term "spurious relationship" is commonly used in statistics and in particular in experimental research techniques, both of which attempt to understand and predict direct causal relationships (X → Y). A non-causal correlation can be spuriously created by an antecedent which causes both (W → X and W → Y). Intervening variables (X → W → Y), if undetected, may make indirect causation look direct. Because of this, experimentally identified correlations do not represent causal relationships unless spurious relationships can be ruled out.
Experiments
In experiments, spurious relationships can often be identified by controlling for other factors, including those that have been theoretically identified as possible confounding factors. For example, consider a researcher trying to determine whether a new drug kills bacteria; when the researcher applies the drug to a bacterial culture, the bacteria die. But to help in ruling out the presence of a confounding variable, another culture is subjected to conditions that are as nearly identical as possible to those facing the first-mentioned culture, but the second culture is not subjected to the drug. If there is an unseen confounding factor in those conditions, this control culture will die as well, so that no conclusion of efficacy of the drug can be drawn from the results of the first culture. On the other hand, if the control culture does not die, then the researcher cannot reject the hypothesis that the drug is efficacious.
Non-experimental statistical analyses
Primarily non-experimental disciplines such as economics usually employ pre-existing data rather than experimental data to establish causal relationships and to determine that they are not spurious. The body of statistical techniques that are used in economics is referred to as econometrics, and involves substantial use of multivariate regression analysis. Typically a linear relationship such as
- y = a0 + a1x1 + a2x2 + ... + akxk + e
is postulated, in which y is the dependent variable (hypothesized to be the caused variable), xj for j=1,...,k is the jth independent variable (hypothesized to be a causative variable), and e is the error term (containing the combined effects of all other causative variables, which must be uncorrelated with the included independent variables). If there is reason to believe that none of the xjs is caused by y, then estimates of the coefficients aj are obtained. If the null hypothesis that aj = 0 is rejected, then the alternative hypothesis that and equivalently that xj causes y cannot be rejected. On the other hand, if the null hypothesis that aj = 0 cannot be rejected, then equivalently the hypothesis of no causal effect of xj on y cannot be rejected. Here the notion of causality is one of contributory causality: If the true value , then a change in xj will result in a change in y unless some other causative variable(s), either included in the regression or implicit in the error term, change in such a way as to exactly offset its effect; thus a change in xj is not sufficient to change y. Likewise, a change in xj is not necessary to change y, because a change in y could be caused by something implicit in the error term (or by some other causative explanatory variable included in the model).
Regression analysis controls for other relevant variables by including them as regressors (explanatory variables). This helps to avoid false inferences of causality due to the presence of a third, underlying, variable that influences both the potentially causative variable and the potentially caused variable: its affect on the potentially caused variable is captured by directly including it in the regression, so that effect will not be picked up as a spurious effect of the potentially causative variable of interest. In addition, the use of multivariate regression helps to avoid wrongly inferring that an indirect effect of, say x1 (e.g., x1 → x2 → y) is a direct effect (x1 → y).
Just as an experimenter must be careful to control for every confounding factor, by holding such factors constant throughout the experiment, so also must the user of multiple regression be careful to control for every confounding factor by including them as xj variables in the regression. If a confounding factor is omitted from the regression, it exists by default in the error term, and if the latter is correlated with one (or more) of the included explanators then the regression results may be spurious.
See also
Footnotes
References
- Pearl, Judea. Causality: Models, Reasoning and Inference, Cambridge University Press, 2000.
- Yule, G.U, 1926, "Why do we sometimes get nonsense correlations between time series? A study in sampling and the nature of time series", Journal of the Royal Statistical Society 89, 1–64.
External links
- Burns, William C., "Spurious Correlations", 1997.
- "The Art and Science of Cause and Effect": a slide show and tutorial lecture by Judea Pearl
Categories:- Logical fallacies
- Logic and statistics
- Statistical dependence
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.