Internal validity

Internal validity

Internal validity is the validity of (causal) inferences in scientific studies, usually based on experiments as experimental validity [ Mitchell, M. and Jolley, J. (2001). Research Design Explained (4th Ed) New York:Harcourt.] .

Details

Inferences are said to possess internal validity if a causal relation between two variables is properly demonstrated [ Brewer, M. (2000). Research Design and Issues of Validity. In Reis, H. and Judd, C. (eds.) Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.] [ Shadish, W., Cook, T., and Campbell, D. (2002). Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generilized Causal Inference Boston:Houghton Mifflin.] . A causal inference may be based on a relation when three criteria are satisfied:
# the "cause" precedes the "effect" in time (temporal precedence),
# the "cause" and the "effect" are related (covariation), and
# there are no plausible alternative explanations for the observed covariation (nonspuriousness) [ ibid. ] .

In scientific experimental settings, researchers often manipulate a variable (the independent variable) to see what effect it has on a second variable (the dependent variable) [ Levine, G. and Parkinson, S. (1994). Experimental Methods in Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum.] For example, a researcher might, for different experimental groups, manipulate the dosage of a particular drug between groups to see what effect it has on health. In this example, the researcher wants to make a causal inference, namely, that different doses of the drug may be "held responsible" for observed changes or differences. When the researcher may confidently attribute the observed changes or differences in the dependent variable to the independent variable, and when he can rule out other explanations (or "rival hypotheses"), then his causal inference is said to be internally valid [Liebert, R. M. & Liebert, L. L. (1995). Science and behavior: An introduction to methods of psychological research. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.] .

In many cases, however, the magnitude of effects found in the dependent variable may not just depend on
* variations in the independent variable,
* the power of the instruments and statistical procedures used to measure and detect the effects, and
* the choice of statistical methods (see: Statistical conclusion validity).

Rather, a number of variables or circumstances uncontrolled for (or uncontrollable) may lead to additional or alternative explanations (a) for the effects found and/or (b) for the magnitude of the effects found. Internal validity, therefore, is more a matter of degree than of either-or, and that is exactly why research designs other than true experiments may also yield results with a high degree of Internal Validity.

In order to allow for inferences with a high degree of internal validity, precautions may be taken during the design of the scientific study. As a rule of thumb, conclusions based on correlations or associations may only allow for lesser degrees of internal validity than conclusions drawn on the basis of direct manipulation of the independent variable. And, when viewed only from the perspective of Internal Validity, highly controlled true experimental designs (i.e. with random selection, random assignment to either the control or experimental groups, reliable instruments, reliable manipulation processes, and safeguards against confounding factors) may be the "gold standard" of scientific research. By contrast, however, the very strategies employed to control these factors may also limit the generalizability or External Validity of the findings.

Threats to internal validity

Confounding

A major threat to the validity of causal inferences is Confounding: Changes in the dependent variable may rather be attributed to the existence or variations in the degree of a third variable which is related to the manipulated variable. Where Spurious relationships cannot be ruled out, rival hypothesis to the original causal inference hypothesis of the researcher may be developed.

election (bias)

Selection bias refers to the problem that, at pre-test, differences between groups exist that may interact with the independent variable and thus be 'responsible' for the observed outcome. Researchers and participants bring to the experiment a myriad of characteristics, some learned and others inherent. For example, sex, weight, hair eye, and skin color, personality, mental capabilities, and physical abilities, but also attitudes like motivation or willingness to participate.

During the selection step of the research study, if an unequal number of test subjects have similar subject-related variables there is a threat to the internal validity. For example, a researcher created two test groups, the experimental and the control groups. The subjects in both groups are not alike with regard to the independent variable but similar in one or more of the subject-related variables. It would be difficult for the researcher to determine if the discrepancy in the groups is due to the independent variable or to the subject-related variables. Selection bias may be reduced when selection/inclusion processes are controlled for and group assignment is randomized. However, in most cases, it may never be ruled out completely as relevant between-group differences may go unnoticed.

History

Events outside of the study/experiment or between repeated measures of the dependent variable may affect participants' responses to experimental procedures. Often, these are large scale events (natural disaster, political change, etc) that affect participants' attitudes and behaviors such that it becomes impossible to determine whether any change on the dependent measures is due to the independent variable, or the historical event.

Maturation

Subjects change during the course of the experiment or even between measurements. For example, young children might mature and their ability to concentrate may change as the grow up. Both permanent changes, such as physical growth and temporary ones like fatigue, provide "natural" alternative explanations; thus, they may change the way a subject would react to the dependent variable. So upon completion of the study, the researcher may not be able to determine if the cause of the discrepancy is due to time or the independent variable.

Repeated testing

Repeatedly measuring the participants may lead to bias. Participants may remember the correct answers or may be conditioned to know that they are being tested. Repeately taking (the same or similar) intelligence tests usually leads to score gains, but instead of concluding that the underlying skills have changed for good, this threat to Internal Validity provides good rival hypotheses.

Instrument change

The instrument used during the testing process can change the experiment. This also refers to observers being more concentrated or primed. If any instrumentation changes occur, the internal validity of the main conclusion is affected, as alternative explanations are readily available.

Regression toward the mean

This type of error occurs when subjects are selected on the basis of extremes score (one far away from the mean) during the first test but score closer to the mean with the second test. For example, when in 3rd grade children with the worst reading skills are selected to participate in a nationwide reading skills program, may it be validly concluded that positive changes at the end of the program are due to the educational efforts? A good alternative conclusion is regression toward the mean and occurs due to a regression artifact. If the children had been tested again before the start of the program, they would likely have obtained less extreme scores, even if repeated testing effects could have been ruled out.

Mortality/differential attrition

This error occurs if inferences are made on the basis of only those participants that have participated from the start to the end. However, participants may have dropped out of the study before completion, and maybe even due to the study or programme or experiment itself. For example, the percentage of group members having quit smoking at post-test was found much higher in a group having received a quit-smoking training program than in the control group. However, in the experimental group only 60% have completed the program. If this attrition is systematically related to any feature of the study, the administration of the independent variable, the instrumentation, or if dropping out leads to relevant bias between groups, a whole class of alternative explanations is possible that account for the observed differences.

election-maturation interaction

This occurs when the subject-related variables, color of hair, skin color, etc., and the time-related variables, age, physical size, etc., interact. In the fruit experiment, the ages of the children in one school is 4-12 and the other 4-9 years old. If a discrepancy between the two groups occurs between the testing, the discrepancy may be due to the age differences in the age categories.

Diffusion

If treatment effects spread from treatment groups to control groups, a lack of differences between experimental and control groups may be observed. This does not mean, however, that the independent variable has no effect or that there is no relationship between dependent and independent variable.

Compensatory rivalry/resentful demoralization

Behaviour in the control groups may alter as a result of the study. For example, control group members may work extra hard to see that expected superiority of the experimental group is not demonstrated. Again, this does not mean, that the independent variable produced no effect or that there is no relationship between dependent and independent variable. Vice-versa, changes in the dependent variable may only be effected due to a demoralized control group, working less hard or motivated, not due to the independent variable.

Experimenter bias

Experimenter bias occurs when the individuals who are conducting an experiment inadvertently affect the outcome by non-consciously behaving differently to members of control and experimental groups. It is possible to eliminate the possibility of experimenter bias through the use of double blind study designs, in which the experimenter is not aware of the condition to which a participant belongs.

ee also

*External validity
*Construct validity
*Content validity
*Statistical conclusion validity
*Validity in statistics
*Ecological validity

References


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужен реферат?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • internal validity — the extent to which the effects detected in a study are truly caused by the treatment or exposure in the study sample, rather than being due to other biasing effects of extraneous variables …   Medical dictionary

  • Validity (statistics) — In psychology, validity has two distinct fields of application. The first involves test validity, a concept that has evolved with the field of psychometrics but which textbooks still commonly gloss over in explaining that it is the degree to… …   Wikipedia

  • Внутренняя валидность (internal validity) — В. в. относится к степени уверенности, с к рой можно судить о предполагаемой каузальной связи между переменными. Эксперимент обладает В. в. в той мере, в какой наблюдаемые эффекты можно отнести к влиянию независимых переменных, а не к. л. др.… …   Психологическая энциклопедия

  • Internal control — In accounting and organizational theory, Internal control is defined as a process effected by an organization s structure, work and authority flows, people and management information systems, designed to help the organization accomplish specific… …   Wikipedia

  • Internal Revenue Service — Infobox Government agency agency name = Internal Revenue Service nativename = IRS abbreviation = IRS logo width = 160px logo caption = seal width = 140px seal caption = formed = July 9, 1953 preceding1 = Bureau of Internal Revenue jurisdiction =… …   Wikipedia

  • internal study validity — vidinis tyrimo pagrįstumas statusas T sritis biomedicinos mokslai apibrėžtis Tyrimo apibūdinimas, rodantis tirtos grupės nustatytų dėsningumų tikrumą. Vidinį tyrimo pagrįstumą mažina ↑sisteminės ir ↑atsitiktinės klaidos. atitikmenys: angl.… …   Lithuanian dictionary (lietuvių žodynas)

  • Internal consistency of the Bible — An American family Bible dating to 1859. The question of the internal consistency of the Bible concerns the coherence and textual integrity of the Biblical scriptures. This has long been an issue for Christians and Jews, who consider the Bible… …   Wikipedia

  • External validity — is the validity of generalized (causal) inferences in scientific studies, usually based on experiments as experimental validity. [ Mitchell, M. Jolley, J. (2001). Research Design Explained (4th Ed) New York:Harcourt.] Inferences about cause… …   Wikipedia

  • Pragmatic validity — in research looks to a different paradigms than more traditional, (post)positivistic research approaches. It tries to ameliorate problems associated with the rigour relevance debate, and is applicable in all kinds of research streams. Simply put …   Wikipedia

  • Ecological validity — is a form of validity in a research study. For a research study to possess ecological validity, the methods, materials and setting of the study must approximate the real life situation that is under investigation.Brewer, M. (2000). Research… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”