Missing fundamental

Missing fundamental

A sound is said to have a missing fundamental, suppressed fundamental, or phantom fundamental when its overtones suggest a fundamental frequency but the sound lacks a component at the fundamental frequency itself. However, the brain perceives the pitch of a tone not only by its fundamental frequency, but also by the ratio of the higher harmonics. Thus, we may perceive the same pitch (perhaps with a different timbre) even if the fundamental frequency is missing from a tone.

For example, when a note (that is not a pure tone) has a pitch of 100 Hz, it will consist of frequency components that are integer multiples of that value (e.g. 100, 200, 300, 400, 500.... Hz). However, smaller loudspeakers may not produce low frequencies, and so in our example, the 100 Hz component may be missing. Nevertheless, a pitch corresponding to the fundamental may still be heard.

Contents

Explanation

A low pitch (also known as the pitch of the missing fundamental or virtual pitch) can sometimes be heard when there is no apparent source or component of that frequency. This perception is due to the brain interpreting repetition patterns that are present.[1][2][3]

It was once thought that this effect was because the missing fundamental was replaced by distortions introduced by the physics of the ear. However, experiments subsequently showed that when a noise was added that would have masked these distortions had they been present, listeners still heard a pitch corresponding to the missing fundamental, as reported by J. C. R. Licklider in 1954.[4] It is now widely accepted that the brain processes the information present in the overtones to calculate the fundamental frequency. The precise way in which it does so is still a matter of debate, but the processing seems to be based on an autocorrelation involving the timing of neural impulses in the auditory nerve.

This very concept of "missing fundamental" being reproduced based on the overtones in the tone has been used to create the illusion of bass in sound systems that are not capable of such bass. In mid-1999, Meir Shashoua of Tel Aviv, working for Waves Audio, patented an algorithm to create the sense of the missing fundamental by synthesizing higher harmonics.[5] Waves Audio released the MaxxBass plug-in to allow computer users to apply the synthesized harmonics to their audio files. Later, Waves Audio produced small subwoofers that relied on the missing fundamental concept to give the illusion of low bass.[6] Both products processed certain overtones selectively to help small loudspeakers, ones which could not reproduce low-frequency components, to sound as if they were capable of low bass. Both products included a high-pass filter which greatly attenuated all the low frequency tones that were expected to be beyond the capabilities of the target sound system.[7] One example of a popular song that was recorded with MaxxBass processing is "Lady Marmalade", the 2001 Grammy award-winning version sung by Christina Aguilera, Lil' Kim, Mýa, and Pink, produced by Missy Elliott.[7]

Other software and hardware companies have developed their own versions of missing fundamental-based bass augmentation products. The poor bass reproduction of earbuds has been identified as a possible target for such processing.[8] Many computer sound systems are not capable of low bass, and songs offered to consumers via computer have been identified as ones that may benefit from augmented bass harmonics processing.[9]

Examples

On-line sound examples comparing pure and complex tones and complexes with missing fundamentals can be found here.

Timpani (kettle drums) are tuned by listening for the missing fundamental. Hit in the usual way (half to three-quarters the distance from the center to the rim), the fundamental note of a timpano is very weak in relation to its second through fifth harmonics.[10] A timpano tuned to 100 Hz will produce sound most strongly at 200, 300, 400 and 500 Hz.[11]

A violin's lowest air and body resonances generally fall between 250 Hz and 300 Hz. The fundamental frequency of the open G3 string is below 200 Hz in modern tunings as well as most historical tunings, so the lowest notes of a violin have an attenuated fundamental, although listeners seldom notice this.

Most common telephones cannot reproduce sounds lower than 300 Hz, but a male voice has a fundamental frequency approximately 150 Hz. Because of the missing fundamental effect, male voices still sound male over the telephone.[12]

The missing fundamental phenomenon is used electronically by some pro audio manufacturers to allow sound systems to seem to produce notes that are lower in pitch than they are capable of reproducing.[13] In a hardware effects unit or a software plugin, a crossover filter is set at a low frequency above which the sound system is capable of safely reproducing tones. Musical signal content above the high-pass part of the crossover filter is sent to the main output which is amplified by the sound system. Low frequency content below the low-pass part of the crossover filter is sent to a circuit where harmonics are synthesized above the low notes. The newly created harmonics are mixed back into the main output to create a perception of the filtered-out low notes.[14] Using a device with this synthetic process can reduce complaints from low frequency noise carrying through walls and it can be employed to reduce low frequency content in loud music that might otherwise vibrate and damage breakable valuables.[15]

A pipe organ sometimes makes use of this phenomenon as a resultant tone.

Differences in perception

Research conducted at Heidelberg University, as described in the January 2006 issue of the German audiophile magazine AUDIO, indicates that the general population can be divided into those who perceive missing fundamentals, and those who primarily hear overtones. The magazine article states that the difference between the perceived pitches can be up to 4 octaves.

See also

References

  1. ^ Jan Schnupp, Israel Nelken and Andrew King (2011). Auditory Neuroscience. MIT Press. ISBN 026211318X. https://mustelid.physiol.ox.ac.uk/drupal/?q=topics/how-many-repetitions-are-required-produce-pitch. 
  2. ^ John Clark, Colin Yallop and Janet Fletcher (2007). An Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology. Blackwell Publishing. ISBN 1405130830. http://books.google.com/books?id=dX5P5mxtYYIC&pg=PA233&dq=phantom-fundamental+pitch+perception. 
  3. ^ Christopher J. Plack (2005). Pitch: Neural Coding and Perception. Springer. ISBN 9780387234724. http://books.google.com/books?id=n6VdlK3AQykC&pg=PP1&dq=Pitch#v=onepage&q=&f=false. 
  4. ^ Peter M. Todd and D. Gareth Loy (1991). Music and Connectionism. MIT Press. ISBN 0262200813. http://books.google.com/books?id=NxycaQH6PeoC&pg=PA86&dq=missing-fundamental+pitch+perception+distortion+noise+masked. 
  5. ^ U.S. Patent 5,930,373
  6. ^ Norem, Josh (May 2004). "MaxxBass MiniWoofer". Maximum PC (Future US, Inc.): 78. ISSN 1522-4279. http://books.google.com/books?id=cAIAAAAAMBAJ&pg=PA78&lpg=PA78&dq=%22missing+fundamental%22#v=onepage&q=%22missing%20fundamental%22&f=false. Retrieved May 11, 2010. 
  7. ^ a b Bundschuh, Paul (April 15–17, 2004). "MaxxBass Applications for Small, Full Range Loudspeakers". Loudspeaker University. Nashua, New Hampshire: Waves Audio. http://www.maxx.com/objects/PDF/Loudspeaker_U-MaxxBass_with_High_BL.pdf. Retrieved May 11, 2010. 
  8. ^ Arora, Manish; Seongcheol Jang, Hangil Moon (September 2006). "Low Complexity Virtual Bass Enhancement Algorithm For Portable Multimedia Device". AES Conference (Audio Engineering Society). http://www.aes.org/e-lib/browse.cfm?elib=13868. Retrieved May 11, 2010. 
  9. ^ Houghton, Matt (April 2007). "Better Bass: The Complete Guide To Recording, Mixing & Monitoring The Low End". Sound On Sound. http://www.soundonsound.com/sos/apr07/articles/betterbass.htm. Retrieved May 11, 2010. 
  10. ^ Howard, David M.; Jamie Angus (2006). Acoustics and psychoacoustics. Focal Press. pp. 202. ISBN 0240519957. http://books.google.com/books?id=Yyk5nAIJcBcC. 
  11. ^ McGill University. Physics Department. Guy D. Moore. Lecture 26: Percussion
  12. ^ Mather, George (2006). Foundations of perception. Taylor & Francis. p. 125. ISBN 0863778356. http://books.google.com/books?id=LYA9faq3lt4C&pg=PA125. Retrieved May 11, 2010. 
  13. ^ Waves Car Audio. MaxxBass Bass Enhancement Technology
  14. ^ US Method and system for enhancing quality of sound signal 5930373 
  15. ^ ProSoundWeb. LAB: The Classic Live Audio Board. Re: maxxbass posts by Doug Fowler June 28-29, 2008.

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать реферат

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Fundamental frequency — Vibration and standing waves in a string, The fundamental and the first 6 overtones The fundamental frequency, often referred to simply as the fundamental and abbreviated f0 or F0, is defined as the lowest frequency of a periodic waveform. In… …   Wikipedia

  • Missing Persons — Datos generales Origen Los Angeles, California, Estados Unidos …   Wikipedia Español

  • Fundamental polygon — In mathematics, each closed surface in the sense of geometric topology can be constructed from an even sided oriented polygon, called a fundamental polygon, by pairwise identification of its edges. Fundamental parallelogram defined by a pair of… …   Wikipedia

  • The Missing Shade of Blue — is an example introduced by the Scottish philosopher David Hume to show that it is at least conceivable that the mind can generate an idea without first being exposed to the relevant sensory experience. It is regarded as a problem by philosophers …   Wikipedia

  • sound — sound1 soundable, adj. /sownd/, n. 1. the sensation produced by stimulation of the organs of hearing by vibrations transmitted through the air or other medium. 2. mechanical vibrations transmitted through an elastic medium, traveling in air at a… …   Universalium

  • Sound — /sownd/, n. The, a strait between SW Sweden and Zealand, connecting the Kattegat and the Baltic. 87 mi. (140 km) long; 3 30 mi. (5 48 km) wide. Swedish and Danish, Oresund. * * * I Mechanical disturbance that propagates as a longitudinal wave… …   Universalium

  • Combination tone — A combination tone, also called a sum tone or a difference tone (also occasionally resultant tone), can be any of at least three similar psychoacoustic phenomena. When two tones are played simultaneously, a listener can sometimes perceive an… …   Wikipedia

  • Psychoacoustics — is the study of subjective human perception of sounds. Alternatively it can be described as the study of the psychological correlates of the physical parameters of acoustics. Background Hearing is not a purely mechanical phenomenon of wave… …   Wikipedia

  • Pitch (psychophysics) — Pitch is the property of a sound that allows the construction of melodies; pitches are compared as higher and lower , and are quantified as frequencies (cycles per second, or hertz), corresponding very nearly to the repetition rate of sound waves …   Wikipedia

  • Voice frequency — A voice frequency (VF) or voice band is one of the frequencies, within part of the audio range, that is used for the transmission of speech. In telephony, the usable voice frequency band ranges from approximately 300 Hz to 3400 Hz. It is for this …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”