Norbert Schwarz

Norbert Schwarz
Norbert Schwarz

Born March 28, 1953 (1953-03-28) (age 58)
Germany
Residence U.S.
Nationality German
Fields Psychology, Marketing, Survey Methodology
Institutions University of Michigan
Universität Heidelberg
ZUMA
Known for Feelings as Information
Notable awards Fellow of American Academy of Arts and Sciences, German National Academy of Science "Leopoldina", American Psychological Association, Association for Psychological Science, Society for Consumer Psychology, Society for Experimental Social Psychology, and Society for Personality and Social Psychology; Wilhelm Wundt Medal; Wilhelm Wundt - William James Award; Rackham Distinguished Graduate Mentor Award

Norbert Schwarz is the Charles Horton Cooley Collegiate Professor of Psychology in the Social Psychology program at the University of Michigan at Ann Arbor. He also has appointments as Professor of Marketing at the Ross School of Business, Research Professor in the Program in Survey Methodology and Research Professor at the Institute for Social Research. He received a Ph.D. in sociology from the University of Mannheim, Germany (1980) and a “Habilitation” in psychology from the University of Heidelberg, Germany (1986). He served as Scientific Director of ZUMA, an interdisciplinary social science research center in Mannheim (1987–1992) before moving to the University of Michigan in 1993. He was a Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in Behavioral Sciences at Stanford in 2009-10.

Norbert Schwarz is among the most frequently cited living researchers in Social Psychology[1] and Consumer Psychology. A core theme of his work is that people do not have stable, coherent and readily accessible attitudes that can be reliably measured through self-report. Instead, opinions are constructed on the spot and recent, contextual factors exert a disproportionate influence on judgments. These influences include feelings (such as moods, emotions, and metacognitive experiences), inferences about the meaning implicit in questions, and whether feelings and thoughts are used to form a representation of the target of judgment or the standard against which it is compared.

Contents

Feelings as Information

Norbert Schwarz proposed the ‘feelings-as-information’ hypothesis, one of the most influential explanations for the cognitive consequences of affect.[2] According to this perspective, when people make judgments about a target, they rely upon their feelings as diagnostic information about the target of judgment. Although this generally produces accurate responses, people sometimes make mistakes about the source of this information. This hypothesis is well demonstrated by mood effects where people tend to evaluate various targets more positively when they are in a good mood than in a bad mood. For instance, people report higher life satisfaction when they are in a good mood on a sunny day rather than in a bad mood on a rainy day.[3] However, if the interviewer mentions the weather before they ask the life satisfaction question, this mood effect disappears because people accurately attribute their current mood to the weather rather than their life satisfaction.

In other work from the feelings-as-information perspective, Schwarz suggests that metacognitive experiences, such as the feeling of ease or difficulty in recalling or processing information, can exert significant influence on judgments. In other words, people tend to make judgments based on this interpretation of their subjective feelings of ease or difficulty in information processing. Such feelings can come from a variety of different sources that are irrelevant to a judgment. For example, the feeling of effort can be elicited by contextual features such as the demands of the task (trying to come up with a few versus many exemplars), processing fluency (high or low figure-ground contrast, easy- versus difficult-to-read fonts) and motor movements (brow contraction). Effortful feelings produced by these manipulations can influence judgments about truth, frequency, risk, and beauty: Easy-to-process stimuli are viewed as more accurate, more likely, less risky, and more beautiful.

For instance, his work has shown that people tend to conclude that they are more assertive when they are asked to recall 6 instances of assertive behavior (an easy task), compared to 12 instances of their own assertiveness (a difficult task), even though the people asked to list 12 instances end up generating more examples of assertive behavior. This demonstrates that the meaning of thought content is informed by the experience of thinking about it.[4]

As another example, inferences about familiarity can be drawn from feelings of ease. As a result, when a sentence such as ‘Orsono is a city in Chile,’ is presented in easy-to-read print fonts, people tend to judge it as true more often than when it is presented in hard-to-read print fonts.[5] This effect is presumably driven by people’s inference based on their naïve theory that easily processed statements are likely to have been encountered before, and therefore, are likely to be true.

Gricean Maxims and Survey Response

Norbert Schwarz is also well known for his research on cognitive processes underlying survey response. This work generally treats the survey interview context as a conversation between the researcher and the respondent. According to this logic, surveys are governed by the cooperative principle advanced by Paul Grice, the late philosopher of language. Put simply, the cooperative principle states that people try to communicate clearly and truthfully, in as much detail as required (but not more so), giving only relevant information. In Schwarz's view, the respondent not only follows the Gricean maxims (Quality, Quantity, Relation, and Manner) when responding to surveys but also assumes that the questions the interviewer asks are guided by the same principles.

Schwarz's research implicates the operation of these maxims during various stages of the survey question and answering process, and highlights how features of the research instrument can significantly impact the answers obtained. For example, when asked about how successful their lives have been, 34% of respondents reported that their lives have been highly successful when the response scale was labeled -5 to 5, whereas only 13% reported high success when the scale was labeled 0 to 10.[6] Presumably this is because the survey respondent assumes that negative integers refer to the presence of negative features, while smaller positive integers refer to the absence of positive features.

Similarly, when a question about marital satisfaction precedes a question about general life satisfaction, responses for the two questions are highly correlated because the first question renders information about one's marriage highly accessible. However, this correlation vanishes when the two questions are framed as subordinate parts of a larger question, presumably because the respondent infers that the interviewer does not want redundant information and thus marital satisfaction should be specifically subtracted from general life satisfaction.[7]

Categorization and Judgment

Norbert Schwarz's work on categorization and mental construal led to the development of his inclusion/exclusion model[8] that accounts for the emergence of contrast and assimilation effects in social judgments. Contrast effects occur when exposure to valenced information influences judgments in a way that is incongruent with the valenced information. Assimilation effects occur when exposure to valenced information influences judgments in a way that is congruent with the valenced information. The key insight of the inclusion/exclusion model is that the evaluation of a target of judgment requires bringing to mind both the target itself, and a standard against which it is to be evaluated. Whether valenced information produces contrast or assimilation depends on whether it is included within the target (assimilation) or in the standard against which it is compared (contrast).

Therefore, by manipulating a given piece of information as either included within the target or compared against, the same information can have different consequences for judgments. For example, thinking of a politician involved in a scandal (such as Elliot Spitzer) may make people believe that politicians in general are more corrupt because the corrupt exemplar is information that is included within the representation of "politicians". In short, people would be left thinking "they are all like Spitzer". Paradoxically, at the same time every individual politician that is rated may seem more honest, because for these judgments, the exemplar is used as the standard of comparison. In this case, people are left thinking "he (or she) is not as bad as Spitzer".[9]

References

  1. ^ Tesser, A., & Bau, J. J. (2002). Social psychology: Who we are and what we do. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 72–85.
  2. ^ Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 385-407). New York: Guilford.
  3. ^ Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983) Mood, misattribution and judgement of well-being. Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523
  4. ^ Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202.
  5. ^ Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 8, 338–342.
  6. ^ Schwarz, N., Knauper, B., Hippler, H. J., Noelle-Neumann, E., & Clark, F. (1991). Rating scales: Numeric values may change the meaning of scale labels. Public Opinion Quarterly, 5, 570-582.
  7. ^ Schwarz, N., Strack, F., & Mai, H. P. (1991). Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole question sequences: A conversational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 3-23.
  8. ^ Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (2007). Mental construal processes: The inclusion/exclusion model. In D. A. Stapel & J. Suis (Eds.), Assimilation and contrast in social psychology (pp. 119–142). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
  9. ^ Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Scandals and the public's trust in politicians: Assimilation and contrast effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 574-579.

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем решить контрольную работу

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Norbert Schwarz — (* 28. März 1953) ist Charles Horton Cooley Collegiate Professor für Sozialpsychologie an der University of Michigan in Ann Arbor. Er ist auch Professor für Marketing an der Business School, Forschungsprofessor im Programm für Umfragemethoden und …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Schwarz (Familienname) — Relative Häufigkeit des Familiennamens Schwartz in Deutschland (Stand: Mai 2010) Schwarz ist ein deutscher Familienname. Herkunft und Bedeutung Schwarz ist ein Übername (Eigenschaftsname) nach der Haarfarbe (wie Braun, Weiß). Darüber hinaus ist… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Norbert Röttgen — (2011) Norbert Röttgen (* 2. Juli 1965 in Meckenheim) ist ein deutscher Politiker (CDU). Seit dem 28. Oktober 2009 ist er Bundesminister für Umwelt, Naturschutz und Reaktorsicherheit. Seit dem 6. November 2010 ist er zudem Lande …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Norbert Schmitz — Norbert „Nobbi“ Christian Schmitz (* 27. Dezember 1958 in Düren; † 6. April 1998) war ein deutscher Fußballspieler. Norbert Schmitz begann das Fußballspielen im Alter von 12 Jahren in seinem Heimatdorf beim JVC Columbia 1903 Drove. Der… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Schwarz-Erle — (Alnus glutinosa) Systematik Rosiden Eurosiden I Ordnung …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Norbert Schmitz — Personal information Full name Norbert Christian Schmitz Date of birth 27 December 1958( …   Wikipedia

  • Norbert Winterstein — (* 12. Dezember 1931 in Alt Sivac, Vojvodina) war ein hessischer Politiker (SPD), Oberbürgermeister in Rüsselsheim und Abgeordneter des Hessischen Landtags. Er ist mit der ehemaligen Vizepräsidentin des Hessischen Landtags Veronika Winterstein… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Norbert Buschlinger —  Norbert Buschlinger Spielerinformationen Geburtstag 6. Dezember 1957 Geburtsort Deutschland Position Abwehr Vereine in der Jugend …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Norbert Stöß — (* 1958 in Dresden) ist ein deutscher Schauspieler. Leben Er wuchs in Plauen im Vogtland auf. Seine Ausbildung erhielt er an der Theaterhochschule „Hans Otto“ in Leipzig, wo er am dortigen Schauspielhaus erste Rollen spielte. Während des Studiums …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Norbert Ivangean —  Norbert Ivangean Spielerinformationen Geburtstag 12. Oktober 1954 Geburtsort Deutschland Position Angriff Vereine als Aktiver Jahr …   Deutsch Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”