- Brain Sex
Infobox Book |
name = Brain Sex
title_orig =
translator =
image_caption =
author = Anne Moir and David Jessel
cover_artist =
country =United Kingdom
language = English
series =
genre =Social Science
publisher = Michael Joseph (Penguin)
release_date = 1989
media_type = Print (Paperback )
pages = viii+228+notes
isbn = 9780718128845 (1st ed.)
preceded_by =
followed_by =" Brain Sex" (ISBN 978-0385311830) is a popular book about the
biology of gender , the biological differences between men and women, by Anne Moir (geneticist ) and David Jessel (journalist ), first published by Michael Joseph (a division of Penguin) in 1989.Moir and Jessel provide a great deal of
evidence and discussion throughout the book. They conclude with a summary, which includes the following decisive statement."We find a political and social view — that men and women should be treated equally — somehow dependent upon a belief that men and women are the same. They are not. There is no longer any excuse, save mental indolence, to believe that::
'Many of the generally understood distinctions between the sexes in the more significant areas of role and temperament, not to mention status, have in fact essentially cultural rather than biological bases.' [K Millet, "Sexual Politics", (London: Virago, 1977), p. 28.]
This isn't a prescriptive book; it merely explains how the brains of the sexes are different, and attempts to link those differences with the observably different behaviour of men and women — which men and women have been celebrating or bemoaning for centuries." [Moir and Jessel, "Brain Sex", (Mandarin, 1991): 177.]
Quotes
*To maintain that [men and women] are the same in aptitude, skill, or behaviour is to build a society based on a biological and scientific lie. ["Ibid"., p. 5.]
*Recent decades have witnessed two contradictory processes: the development of scientific research into the differences between the sexes, and the political denial that such differences exist. ["Ibid"., p. 12.]
*There is no point ... in laws and educational theories which both deny the differences between men and women, and then seek to eradicate them. ["Ibid"., p. 190.]Contents
The sequence of chapters broadly follows the human life cycle — birth, maturity, reproduction ... sustained by work.:*Introduction
#Chapter One: The differences
#Chapter Two: The birth of difference
#Chapter Three: Sex in the brain
#Chapter Four: Childhood differences
#Chapter Five: The brains come of age
#Chapter Six: The ability gap
#Chapter Seven: Hearts and minds
#Chapter Eight: Like minds
#Chapter Nine: The marriage of two minds
#Chapter Ten: Why mothers are not fathers
#Chapter Eleven: Minds at work
#Chapter Twelve: Bias at work:*Summary:*References:*Indexummary
Chapter One: The differences
The first chapter of the book establishes the state of the
gender debate at the time of publication. It acknowledges that the majority opinion of educated people (at the time of publication) was that men and women have the same brain. However, it claims that this is in stark contrast to the opinion ofresearch scientist s in the area. There is a cursoryliterature review of a century of sex research, featuring some early crude and unreliable results (the book's assessment), but proceeding to the modern results available from about the middle of the 20th century. ["Ibid"., pp. 12ff.]Chapter Two: The birth of difference
In chapter two, the authors begin their systematic presentation of the scientific
data . It starts by explainingsexual reproduction , thesex determination system and X and Ychromosome s. ["Ibid"., pp. 21ff.] It extends beyond this to late 20th century discoveries regarding the effects of hormones onbrain formation in thewomb . ["Ibid"., p. 24-25.] It gives extended treatment of studies on animals, which show both distinct similarities and differences to humans. ["Ibid"., p. 25-29.] Rat brain structure and consequent behaviour can be altered by hormone treatment "after birth" in ways that have not worked with humans. Four case studies of people who were born withintersex physicalities are used to illustrate the effects of hormonal influence on human behaviour "prior to birth". ["Ibid"., p. 29-37.]Chapter Three: Sex in the brain
In this chapter, the results regarding a "two-fold" operation of hormones in humans are presented. This is described as an initial morphological influence on the brain during
gestation , followed by an "activation" stage atpuberty . ["Ibid"., p. 38.] In other words, hormones have been discovered to be instrumental in shaping initial brain structure, later they stimulate features of these dimorphic brain structures. Hormone activity is not strictly limited to puberty nor to solely reproductive functions.Brain dimorphism, as observably distinct and complicated as it is, theoretically could be of as little significance for
behaviour as, for example, eye-colour. ["Ibid"., p. 38.] So additional material regardingcorrelation s between brain structure and behaviour is actually the main feature of this chapter. Initial studies in this area came from observing people who had sufferedbrain damage . Correlations between which behavioural functions showed impairment and which regions of the brain showed damage led to early results regarding left and rightbrain hemisphere activity. ["Ibid"., pp. 39ff.] Althoughknowledge of this area is still far from complete, a great deal of refinement has been made possible withbrain imaging technology and investment in research projects into human brain function.One simplification of the results is that female brains (the result of default or "normal" developmental pathways) generally distribute processing across diverse regions of the brain. Male brains (
testosterone modified versions of the female brain) are notably more "compartmentalized" and "focused" in their processing. ["Ibid"., pp. 42ff.] This is the science behind the popular language of women typically having a natural aptitude for "multi-tasking", and men seeming to generally adopt "single minded" behavioural strategies.Chapter Four: Childhood differences
Chapter four begins with case studies and proceeds through the chronological sub-sections: babies,
toddler s, pre-school andschool . It is in this section that the superior sensory processing of female brains and the tendency ofgirl s to engage more readily withadult s is introduced. ["Ibid"., p. 55.]Boy s, by comparison, do not seem to observe as much, nor have as strong a preference for inter-personal engagement. However, they talk as much (but to themselves or things), and are generally more "active and wakeful." ["Ibid"., p. 56.] The discussion of these things is supported by references to amother , Gillian, who sought to avoid sexual stereotyping in raising hertwin s Annie and Andrew, only to find, with frustration, that herchild ren resisted her efforts. ["Ibid"., pp. 57ff.]Although the chapter makes the case for objective, universal biological development of sex difference in behaviour, it is explicit in noting that
socialization is also very much part of behavioural development in general. The first case study in the chapter is a report of the sad case of Genie, a girl who was raised in isolation and struggled to developlanguage ability. ["Ibid"., p. 54.] The point of the chapter is that sexually dimorphic behaviours "include" biological influences that are resistant to socialization, ["Ibid"., p. 66.] not that all behaviour or all dimorphic behaviours are purely biological. As the conclusion of the book makes explicit, its thesis is that purely social explanations of gender are inadequate, not that biological explanations are sufficient.Chapter Five: The brains come of age
Structural dimorphism of the brain alone can account for a great deal of the behavioural and other differences between boys and girls. ["Ibid"., p. 67.] However, the impact of higher levels of sex-specific hormones on the already dimorphic brains leads to even more profound differences between mature men and women. ["Ibid"., p. 68-69.] A survey of this phenomenon is the topic of chapter five.
Again this chapter features several case studies. It first considers the influence of female hormones on the behaviour of women. ["Ibid"., pp. 70ff.] The pronounced cyclic rhythm of female hormones, and their widely attested effects, is considered in detail. The case studies are extreme examples. ["Ibid"., pp. 73, 75, 77, 78.] The influence of testosterone on men, especially with regard to aggression, is also noted in this chapter and extends to discussion of higher prevalence of social deviancy among men. ["Ibid"., pp. 79ff.] Although consideration of the extremes and the negative social impact of hormonal influence occupies the majority of the chapter, it concludes with aspects of dimorphic patterns of behaviour that are generally considered socially constructive, even if stereotypical. ["Ibid"., pp. 85ff.]
Chapter Six: The ability gap
Chapter six considers the statistical differences in performance between men and women, particularly those of children and adolescents in educational environments. ["Ibid"., pp. 89ff.] Case studies of hormonally atypical children whose performance correlates more closely with children of the other sex is presented. ["Ibid"., pp. 91ff.]
Although there is evidence for measurable differences in performance of various cognitive and other tasks, the chapter focuses on these as an outcome of preference and strategy. It is not a question of seeking to demonstrate an overall superiority of one sex or the other, rather the evidence is evaluated for what it appears to support regarding biologically prompted alternative approaches to life challenges. ["Ibid"., p. 98.]
The point of this is that men and women have preferences, not simply abilities. So external performance based selection of men and women may well lead to more women than men in certain roles (and vice versa). However, the evidence also suggests that men and women are just as likely to learn and pursue specific social roles in different proportions, by virtue of internal preferences and irrespective of external selection. ["Ibid"., p. 97.]
The specific studies and numbers that are quoted in the chapter mainly focus on male advantages in abstract theoretical reasoning, in particular traditional theoretical mathematics. ["Ibid"., p. 89-91.] The general picture presented is of a male preference to systematize, where the female preference is to sympathize. ["Ibid"., p. 95-97.] These stereotypes are not new, it was the popularization of peer-reviewed study of them that was new.
Chapter Seven: Hearts and minds
This chapter considers the differences in erotic preferences and behaviour between men and women. It starts by introducing difference in expressed preferences for certain physical features, ["Ibid"., p. 99.] but then turns to psychological and relational aspects. ["Ibid"., pp. 100f.] After some caveats regarding the subjectivity of previous work on sexual anthropology, ["Ibid"., pp. 102f.] treatment of scientific studies of hormonal influences on libido is introduced. ["Ibid"., pp. 103f.] The chapter includes treatment of well-known generalizations like: men being more sexually driven than women, ["Ibid"., p. 102 and others.] arousal being visual for men and by touch for women, ["Ibid"., p. 106.] and men wanting sex where women want a relationship. ["Ibid"., p. 107 and others.] Hormonal explanations for these tendencies are offered, and reference to studies where available. There is a sustained conversational style of presentation in this chapter, which continually returns to the practical difficulties of misunderstandings arising in male-female relationships due to different expectations, especially if the differences between the sexes are denied.
Chapter Eight: Like minds
Treatment of like mindedness in couples is actually the consistent theme of chapter seven. In this chapter, it is homosexual couples that are on view, in particular male homosexuals — in other words sexual partners with similarly structured brains. Male homosexuals are particularly on view because it would appear that homosexuality in men and women is motivated and expressed quite differently. It would also appear that male homosexuality has been considerably more studied than female homosexuality. The chapter features the work of an East German scientist, Gunter Dörner, from the
cold war era. It concludes with a quote fromSigmund Freud — "bear in mind that some day all our provisional formulations in psychology will have to be based on an organic foundation . . . It will then probably be seen that it is special chemical substances and processes which achieve the effects of sexuality." [Sigmund Freud, location not provided in Moir and Jessel.]Chapter Nine: The marriage of two minds
This chapter addresses the issue of marital conflict, which it believes to be inevitable, but not fatal. Men and women typically express different goals in life generally, and in personal relationship in particular. The main concern of the chapter is that by educating boys and girls in the same things, and in teaching them that they are the same, society is undermining their future marriages. ["Ibid"., p. 127.] Essentially the chapter deconstructs modern assumptions regarding equality and power in interpersonal relationships. This is treated mainly from the perspective of women's frustrations within marriages — "the inequality of the emotional contract." ["Ibid"., p. 135.] It is seen as an inevitability, only made more painful by denial. The chapter concludes by summarizing its warning, while reasserting the complementary diversity it sees as a constructive feature of sexual differences, if embraced. "Marriages go wrong when men and women fail to acknowledge, or begin to resent, each other's complementary differences." ["Ibid"., p. 140.]
Chapter Ten: Why mothers are not fathers
There are two main themes to this chapter: the complexity and demanding nature of parenting, and the advantages the typical female brain seems to have in applying itself to the challenges. The chapter is explicit in addressing motherhood and fatherhood, not gender-neutral "parenthood". It also addresses both a widespread desire among women to be mothers, and the common experience of guilt among women regarding time spent away from their children. The evolution of social structures in the Israeli
kibutz is reported, where despite an ideological commitment to community, rather than family, investment in raising children, it is the women who have sought and regained the role of motherhood. There is only limited treatment of the role of fatherhood in this chapter.Chapter Eleven: Minds at work
The main point of chapter eleven is that men and women appear to perceive success differently, they articulate different ambitions. A comparison between the priorities of male and female academics is given. The men, in general, seek publication and status, where the women seek to provide quality education for their students. ["Ibid"., p. 157.] Several examples of studies along these lines are presented in the body of the chapter. "Both" publication "and" personal coaching of students are important contributions to the vitality of educational institutions within the field of academia. The chapter concludes by suggesting that in many fields, the distinctive contributions of typical men and women are actually complementary. Combining the different patterns of talent, rather than denying their existence, is suggested to be a more logical approach to increased productivity in the workplace. ["Ibid"., p. 164.]
Chapter Twelve: Bias at work
The final chapter of the book argues first that the typical female preference and aptitude for "personal relationships" suits them to employment in workplace roles that capitalize on this. ["Ibid"., pp. 166ff.] It continues this line of thinking in a creative way, suggesting we have a genuine social problem regarding stereotyping in the workplace; but this problem is not the problem of stereotyping the "sexes", rather it is the problem of stereotyping the "roles". Management and decision making stereotypes are discussed, and it is suggested that particular, "macho" stereotypes predominate. If we want female managers, we should be allowing them to manage in feminine ways, and we might just find these to be workable, and even superior in certain contexts. ["Ibid"., pp. 168ff.]
References
The reference section of the book is divided into several sections: "General References" (a page-and-a-half), references by chapter (fifteen pages of bibliography), and twelve books described as "Summary References". These sections are followed by eight-and-a-half pages of citations for the end notes marked in the main text.
Reviews
*"This book is a thoroughly good read, couched comfortably in layman's language and carrying the reader on at a spanking pace." — Glasgow Herald
*"For the past 30 years we have been told that men and women are interchangeable in every way. Now a sensational book explodes the myth of sexual sameness." —Daily Mail [http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2006.00180.x?cookieSet=1&journalCode=jsm 'Brain Sex—Testosterone Role in Personality Evolution; Females and Males Are Different?'] "The Journal of Sexual Medicine" 3 (2006): 81-82. [http://www.natall.com/national-vanguard/116/bookreviews.html 'Brain Sex'.] National Vanguard Magazine 116 (August-September 1996)ee also
*
Anne Fausto-Sterling
*David Reimer
*Defeminization
*Dihydrotestosterone
*John Money
*Leonard Sax
*Milton Diamond
*Ralph Holloway
*Sexual identity
*Simon Baron-Cohen
*Steven Goldberg
*Testosterone poisoning
*The Inevitability of Patriarchy
*Virilization
*Why Men Rule References
External links
* [http://www.theabsolute.net/misogyny/brainsx.html Excerpts from "Brain Sex"]
*BBC [http://www.bbc.co.uk/science/humanbody/sex Brain Sex test]Literature
*Kimura, Doreen. "Sex and Cognition".
MIT Press , 1999.
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.