Chromatic adaptation

Chromatic adaptation

Chromatic adaptation is one aspect of vision that may fool someone into observing a color-based optical illusion, such as the same color illusion.

An object may be viewed under various conditions. For example, it may be illuminated by sunlight, the light of a fire, or a harsh electric light. In all of these situations, human vision perceives that the object has the same color: an apple always appears red, whether viewed at night or during the day. On the other hand, a camera with no adjustment for light may register the apple as having varying color. This feature of the visual system is called chromatic adaptation, or color constancy; when the correction occurs in a camera it is referred to as white balance.

Though the human visual system generally does maintain constant perceived color under different lighting, there are situations where the relative brightness of two different stimuli will appear reversed at different illuminance levels. For example, the bright yellow petals of flowers will appear dark compared to the green leaves in dim light while the opposite is true during the day. This is known as the Purkinje effect, and arises because the peak sensitivity of the human eye shifts toward the blue end of the spectrum at lower light levels.

Contents

Von Kries transform

The von Kries chromatic adaptation method is a technique that is sometimes used in camera image processing. The method is to apply a gain to each of the human cone cell spectral sensitivity responses so as to keep the adapted appearance of the reference white constant. The application of Johannes von Kries's idea of adaptive gains on the three cone cell types was first explicitly applied to the problem of color constancy by Herbert E. Ives,[1][2] and the method is sometimes referred to as the Ives transform[3] or the von Kries–Ives adaptation.[4]

The von Kries coefficient rule rests on the assumption that color constancy is achieved by individually adapting the gains of the three cone responses, the gains depending on the sensory context, that is, the color history and surround. Thus, the cone responses c' from two radiant spectra can be matched by appropriate choice of diagonal adaptation matrices D1 and D2:[5]

c'=D_1\,S^T\,f_1 = D_2\,S^T\,f_2

where S is the cone sensitivity matrix and f is the spectrum of the conditioning stimulus. This leads to the von Kries transform for chromatic adaptation in LMS color space (responses of long-, medium-, and short-wavelength cone response space):

D = D_1^{-1} D_2=\begin{bmatrix} L_2/L_1 & 0 & 0 \\ 0 & M_2/M_1 & 0 \\ 0 & 0 & S_2/S_1 \end{bmatrix}

This diagonal matrix D maps cone responses, or colors, in one adaptation state to corresponding colors in another; when the adaptation state is presumed to be determined by the illuminant, this matrix is useful as an illuminant adaptation transform. The elements of the diagonal matrix D are the ratios of the cone responses (Long, Medium, Short) for the illuminant's white point.

The more complete von Kries transform, for colors represented in XYZ or RGB color space, includes matrix transformations into and out of LMS space, with the diagonal transform D in the middle.[6]

References

  1. ^ Ives HE (1912). "The relation between the color of the illuminant and the color of the illuminated object". Trans. Illuminat. Eng. Soc. 7: 62–72.  (Reprinted in: Brill, Michael H. (1995). "The relation between the color of the illuminant and the color of the illuminated object". Color Res. Appl. 20: 70–5. doi:10.1002/col.5080200112. )
  2. ^ Hannah E. Smithson and Qasim Zaidi (2004). "Colour constancy in context: Roles for local adaptation and levels of reference". Journal of Vision 4 (9): 693–710. doi:10.1167/4.9.3. PMID 15493964. http://www.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/3. 
  3. ^ Hannah E. Smithson (2005). "Review. Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human color constancy". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 360 (1458): 1329–46. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1633. PMC 1609194. PMID 16147525. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1609194. 
  4. ^ Karl R. Gegenfurtner, L. T. Sharpe (1999). Color Vision: From Genes to Perception. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 052100439X. http://books.google.com/?id=9R1ogJsPHi8C&pg=PA413&dq=von-kries+ives. 
  5. ^ Gaurav Sharma (2003). Digital Color Imaging Handbook. CRC Press. 
  6. ^ Erik Reinhard (2006). High Dynamic Range Imaging: Acquisition, Display, and Image-Based Lighting. Morgan Kaufmann. ISBN 0125852630. http://books.google.com/?id=dH2lRxTg1UsC&pg=PA39&dq=von-kries-transform. 

Further reading

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Chromatic (disambiguation) — Chromatic, a word ultimately derived from the Greek noun χρῶμα (khrṓma), which means complexion or color , and then from the Greek adjective χρωματικός (khrōmatikós; colored ), may refer to: In music: Chromatic scale, the western tempered twelve… …   Wikipedia

  • Color vision — White light shone onto a green surface is perceived as green by the human eye, and processed as such in the brain s visual cortex …   Wikipedia

  • Color balance — This article is about the process applied to still images. For the equivalent process applied to video, see Color grading. The left half shows the photo as it came from the digital camera. The right half shows the photo adjusted to make a gray… …   Wikipedia

  • Color rendering index — Emitted light spectrum determines the color rendering index of the lamp. Color render …   Wikipedia

  • CIECAM02 — Published in 2002 by the CIE Technical Committee 8 01 ( Colour Appearance Modelling for Color Management Systems ), as of 2008 CIECAM02 is the most recent color appearance model ratified by the CIE, and the successor of CIECAM97s.cite… …   Wikipedia

  • LMS color space — LMS is a color space represented by the response of the three types of cones of the human eye, named after their responsivity (sensitivity) at long, medium and short wavelengths.It is common to use the LMS color space when performing chromatic… …   Wikipedia

  • McCollough effect — (click to enlarge) A test image for the McCollough effect. On first looking at this image, the vertical and horizontal lines should look black and white, colourless. After induction (see images below), the horizontal lines should look greenish… …   Wikipedia

  • CIELUV color space — In colorimetry, the CIE 1976 ( L* , u* , v* ) color space, also known as the CIELUV color space, is a color space adopted by the International Commission on Illumination (CIE) in 1976, as a simple to compute transformation of the 1931 CIE XYZ… …   Wikipedia

  • Color — This article is about the perceptual property. For other uses, see Color (disambiguation). For usage of color on Wikipedia, see Wikipedia:Colors. Colored pencils Color or colour (see spelling differences) is the visual perceptual property… …   Wikipedia

  • Pigment — For the drug referred to as pigment, see black tar heroin. Natural ultramarine pigment in powdered form …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”