Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study

Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study

The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study examined the IQ test scores of 130 black/interracial children adopted by advantaged white families. The aim of the study was to determine the contribution of environmental and genetic factors to the poor performance of black children on IQ tests as compared to white children. The initial study was published in 1976[1] by Sandra Scarr and Richard A. Weinberg. A follow-up study was published in 1992[2] by Richard Weinberg, Sandra Scarr and Irwin D. Waldman. Another related study investigating social adjustment in a subsample of the adopted black children was published in 1996.[3]

Contents

Background and study design

On measures of cognitive ability (IQ tests) and school performance, black children in the U.S. perform worse than white children. The gap in average performance between the two groups of children is approximately one standard deviation, which is equivalent to about 15 IQ points or 4 grade levels at high school graduation. Thus, the average IQ score of black children in the U.S. is approximately 85, compared to the average score of white children of 100. No detectable bias due to test construction or administration has been found, although this does not rule out other biases. The gap is functionally significant, which makes it an important area of study. The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study tried to answer whether the gap is primarily caused by genetic factors or whether it is primarily caused by environmental and cultural factors.

The study was funded by the Grant Foundation and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

By examining the cognitive ability and school performance of both black and white children adopted into white families, the study intended to separate genetic factors from rearing conditions as causal influences in the gap. "Trans racial adoption is the human analog of the cross-fostering design, commonly used in animal behavior genetics research.... There is no question that adoption constitutes a massive intervention" (Scarr & Weinberg, 1976, p. 726).

Scarr and Weinberg studied black, white, Asian, indigenous American, and mixed-race black/white children adopted by upper-middle-class white families in Minnesota. The average IQ of the adopting parents was more than one standard deviation above the population mean of 100. The biological children of these parents were also tested. The sample of adopted children was selected by eligible parents contacting the researchers for participating following a newsletter call. The geographical origin of the adopted children was not uniform. All except one white adopted child was adopted in-state. Black and interracial children came from twelve states and Asian and indigenous American children came from Minnesota as well as from Korea, Vietnam, Canada and Ecuador.

As Scarr & Weinberg (1976) note, transracial adoption studies only control for family environment, not social environment. For example, children who are socially identified as black may still be subject to racial discrimination despite being raised by white parents. Yet, it was previously known that adoption into upper-middle class white families has a positive influence on the IQ and school performance of white children.

The study showed significant differences in adoption patterns of mixed-race black/white and black adopted children as was noted by Scarr and Weinberg (1976):

It is essential to note, however, that the groups also differed significantly (p < .05) in their placement histories and natural mother's education. Children with two black parents were significantly older at adoption, had been in the adoptive home a shorter time, and had experienced a greater number of preadoption placements. The natural parents of the black/black group also averaged a year less of education than those of the black/white group, which suggests an average difference between the groups in intellectual ability. There were also significant differences between the adoptive families of black/black and black/white children in father's education and mother's IQ.[1]

Results

The children were first tested in 1975 at age 7. In 1985, 196 of the original 265 children were retested at age 17.

Children's background Age 7 IQ Age 17 IQ Age 17 GPA Age 17 class rank (percentile) Age 17 school aptitude (percentile)
Adopting parents tested when children were 7 and 17 120 115
Non adopted, with two white biological parents 116 109 3.0 64 69
Adopted, with two white biological parents 118 106 2.8 54 59
Adopted, with one white and one black biological parent 110 99 2.2 40 53
Adopted, with Asian or indigenous American parents 101 96
Adopted, with two black biological parents 95 89 2.1 36 42

The adopting parents of 12 of the interracial children wrongly believed that their adopted children had two black parents. The average IQ of these 12 children was not significantly different from the scores of the 56 interracial children correctly classified by their adoptive parents as having one black and one white parent.[1]

The study found that age at adoption was significantly associated with measured IQs:

Children's background Age 7 IQ Age 17 IQ
Early placed adopted children 111 99
Late placed adopted children 97 92

Some have suggested that differing pre-adoption experiences, including age at adoption, explain the racial patterns in the results. Lee (2009) argues against this interpretation, pointing out that there is no evidence from other studies that variables such as age at adoption exert an effect on IQ lasting until late adolescence. In the Minnesota study, the proportion of IQ variance associated with pre-adoption variables declined from .32 to .13 between ages 7 and 17. Lee further suggests that causality may run from IQ and other behavioral variables to differences in pre-adoption experiences rather than the other way around, and that race by itself as a visible characteristic may have affected pre-adoption experience.[4]

The average difference in IQ scores between the testing at age 7 and testing at age 17, seen in all groups, may be due to the use of different IQ tests[2]. The original study used Stanford-Binet Form L-M, WISC or WAIS tests, depending on age, while the follow-up used WISC-R or WAIS-R. Weinberg, Scarr and Waldman point describe the effect of this change in test:

Declines in IQ scores have been documented when individuals are retested on a revised form of an original measure, as well as when a test used at a first administration was normed earlier than a test used at a subsequent administration (see Flynn, 1984, for a review). For example, the decline in Full-Scale IQ score from the WAIS to the WAIS-R averaged 6.8 points across a number of studies (reviewed by Sattler, 1988) and was 7.5 points in a sample of 72 35-44-year-olds tested as part of the standardization of the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981). This is precisely the test combination used for adoptive parents in our study.

Furthermore the data needed to be corrected for the Flynn effect as stated by Ulrich Neisser:

Everyone involved in this debate is well-aware that such comparisons must be corrected for the Flynn effect: Mean scores on all standard IQ tests seem to rise steadily at about 0.3 points per year. In the Minnesota study, where the tests used in the follow-up were generally not the same as those that had been given the first time, these corrections are complex and must be made on an individual basis. Until they have been made–Waldman et al. reported that they are in progress–raw figures like those above are relatively meaningless.[5]

The data corrected for the Flynn effect was published in 2000 by John Loehlin in the Handbook of Intelligence.[6]

Children's background Number of Children Age 7 Corrected IQ Age 17 Corrected IQ
Non adopted, with two white biological parents 101 110.5 105.5
Adopted, with two white biological parents 16 111.5 101.5
Adopted, with one white and one black biological parent 55 105.4 93.2
Adopted, Asian or indigenous American parents 12 96.1 91.2
Adopted, with two black biological parents 21 91.4 83.7

The analysis of structured interviews at age 7 and 17 reported by (DeBerry, Scarr & Weinberg 1996) found that about half of the studied black adopted children had adjustment difficulties. They had difficulties becoming competent in both European and African-American reference group orientation but had stronger affinity with European rather than African-American group. Stronger identification with one or the other group predicted better adjustment.[3].

Interpretations

Scarr & Weinberg (1976) interpreted the results from age 7 suggesting that racial group differences in IQ are inconclusive due to confounding of the study. They noted however that the study indicated that cross-racial adoptive had a positive effect on black adopted children. In support of this interpretation, they drew special attention to the finding that the average IQ of "socially classified" black children was greater than the U.S. white mean. The followup data was collected in 1986 and Weinberg et al. published their findings in 1992 and interpreted their results still supporting the original conclusions.

Both Levin (1994) and Lynn (1994) argued that the data clearly supports a hereditarian alternative: that the mean IQ scores and school achievement of each group reflected their degree of African ancestry. For all measures, the children with two black parents scored lower than the children with one black and white parent, who in turn scored lower than the adopted children with two white parents. Both omitted discussion of Asian adoptees.

Waldman, Weinberg, and Scarr (1994) responded to Levin (1994) and Lynn (1994).[7] They noted that the data taken of adoption placement effects can explain the observed differences. But that they cannot make that claim firmly due to the pre adoption factors confounded racial ancestry, preventing an unambiguous interpretation of the results. They also note that Asian data fits that hypothesis while being omitted by both Levin and Lynn. They argued that,"contrary to Levin's and Lynn's assertions, results from the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study provide little or no conclusive evidence for genetic influences underlying racial differences in intelligence and achievement." and note that "We think that it is exceedingly implausible that these differences are either entirely genetically based or entirely environmentally based. The true causes of racial-group differences in IQ, or in any other characteristic, are likely to be too complex to be captured by locating them on a single hereditarianism-environmentalism dimension."[7] On the question of Arthur Jensen (1998) examined these studies and reviewed the evidence that adoption does not affect children's IQ scores after age 7.[clarification needed]

In a 1998 article, Scarr wrote: "The test performance of the Black/Black adoptees [in the study] was not different from that of ordinary Black children reared by their own families in the same area of the country. My colleagues and I reported the data accurately and as fully as possible, and then tried to make the results palatable to environmentally committed colleagues. In retrospect, this was a mistake. The results of the transracial adoption study can be used to support either a genetic difference hypothesis or an environmental difference one (because the children have visible African ancestry). We should have been agnostic on the conclusions [...]."[8]

Loehlin (2000) reiterates the confounding problems of the study and notes that both genetic and environmental interpretations are possible. He further offers another possible explanation of the results, namely unequal prenatal factors: "[O]ne possibility lies in the prenatal environment provided by Black and White biological mothers. The Black-Black group, of course, all had Black mothers. In the Black-White group, virtually all of the birth mothers were White (66 of 68). Willerman and his colleagues found that in interracial couples it made a difference whether the mother was Black or White: The children obtained higher IQs if she was White. They suspected that this difference was due to postnatal environment, but it could, of course, have been in the prenatal one."[9]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. A. (1976). IQ test performance of black children adopted by White families. American Psychologist, 31, 726-739.
  2. ^ a b Weinberg, R. A., Scarr, S., & Waldman, I. D. (1992). The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A follow-up of IQ test performance at adolescence. Intelligence, 16, 117-135.
  3. ^ a b DeBerry, K. M., Scarr, S. and Weinberg, R. A. (1996) Family Racial Socialization and Ecological Competence: Longitudinal Assessments of African-American Transracial Adoptees. Child Development, 67, 2375-2399.
  4. ^ Lee, J.J. (2009). Review of Intelligence And How to Get It: Why Schools And Cultures Count, R.E. Nisbett, Norton, New York, NY (2009). ISBN: 9780393065053. Personality and Individual Differences 48, 247-255.
  5. ^ Neisser, U. (1997). "Never a Dull Moment". American Psychologist 52 (1): 79–81. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.1.79. http://ist-socrates.berkeley.edu/~maccoun/PP279_Neisser2.html. 
  6. ^ Loehlin, John (2000). "Group Differences in Intelligence". In Robert J. Sternberg. The Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. ; Table 9.3, p 185.
  7. ^ a b Waldman, I. D., Weinberg, R. A., & Scarr, S. (1994). Racial-group differences in IQ in the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A reply to Levin and Lynn. Intelligence, 19, 29-44.
  8. ^ Scarr, S. (1998). On Arthur Jensen's integrity. Intelligence, 26 (3), 227-232.
  9. ^ Loehlin, John (2000). "Group Differences in Intelligence". In Robert J. Sternberg. The Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. ; p 189.
  • Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor. Westport, CT: Praeger.
  • Levin, M. (1994). Comment on Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study. Intelligence, 19, 13-20.
  • Lynn, R. (1994). Some reinterpretations of the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study. Intelligence, 19, 21-27.

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Minnesota Texas Adoption Research Project — The Minnesota / Texas Adoption Research Project (MTARP) is a longitudinal research study that focuses on the consequences of variations in openness in adoption arrangements for all members of the adoptive kinship network: birthmothers, adoptive… …   Wikipedia

  • Race and intelligence — Human intelligence Abilities and Traits …   Wikipedia

  • Intelligent — Intelligenz (lat.: intelligentia „Einsicht, Erkenntnisvermögen“, intellegere „einsehen, verstehen“) bezeichnet im weitesten Sinne die geistige Fähigkeit zum Erkennen von Zusammenhängen und zum Finden von Problemlösungen. Intelligenz kann auch als …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Schlussfolgerndes Denken — Intelligenz (lat.: intelligentia „Einsicht, Erkenntnisvermögen“, intellegere „einsehen, verstehen“) bezeichnet im weitesten Sinne die geistige Fähigkeit zum Erkennen von Zusammenhängen und zum Finden von Problemlösungen. Intelligenz kann auch als …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Intelligenz — (von lat. intellegere „verstehen“, wörtlich „wählen zwischen...“ von lat. inter „zwischen“ und legere „lesen, wählen“) ist in der Psychologie ein Sammelbegriff für die kognitive Leistungsfähigkeit des Menschen. Es gibt keine von allen Psychologen …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • International adoption of South Korean children — The International adoption of South Korean children is a recent historical process triggered initially by casualties of the Korean War after 1953. The initiative was taken by religious organizations in the United States, Australia, and many… …   Wikipedia

  • Hochbegabt — Hochbegabung bezeichnet im Allgemeinen eine umfassende, weit über dem Durchschnitt liegende intellektuelle Begabung eines Menschen. Sie wird durch verschiedene Modelle unterschiedlich definiert. Die Ansätze reichen von sehr engen Fassungen, die… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Hochbegabte — Hochbegabung bezeichnet im Allgemeinen eine umfassende, weit über dem Durchschnitt liegende intellektuelle Begabung eines Menschen. Sie wird durch verschiedene Modelle unterschiedlich definiert. Die Ansätze reichen von sehr engen Fassungen, die… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Hochbegabtenförderung — Hochbegabung bezeichnet im Allgemeinen eine umfassende, weit über dem Durchschnitt liegende intellektuelle Begabung eines Menschen. Sie wird durch verschiedene Modelle unterschiedlich definiert. Die Ansätze reichen von sehr engen Fassungen, die… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Hochbegabung bei Kindern — Hochbegabung bezeichnet im Allgemeinen eine umfassende, weit über dem Durchschnitt liegende intellektuelle Begabung eines Menschen. Sie wird durch verschiedene Modelle unterschiedlich definiert. Die Ansätze reichen von sehr engen Fassungen, die… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”