- Receptive field
The receptive field of a sensory
neuron is a region of space in which the presence of a stimulus will alter the firing of that neuron. Receptive fields have been identified for neurons of theauditory system , thesomatosensory system , and thevisual system .The concept of receptive fields can be extended to further up the neural system; if many sensory receptors all form
synapses with a single cell further up, they collectively form the receptive field of that cell. For example, the receptive field of aganglion cell in theretina of theeye is composed of input from all of thephotoreceptors which synapse with it, and a group of ganglion cells in turn forms the receptive field for a cell in the brain. This process is calledconvergence .Auditory system
In the auditory system, receptive fields can be volumes in auditory space, or can be regions of auditory frequencies. Researchers rarely equate auditory receptive fields to particular regions of the sensory
epithelium such as, in the case ofmammals ,hair cell s in thecochlea .omatosensory system
In the somatosensory system, receptive fields are regions of the
skin or ofinternal organs . Some types ofmechanoreceptor s have large receptive fields, while others have smaller ones.Large receptive fields allow the cell to detect changes over a wider area, but lead to a less precise perception. Thus, the fingers, which require the ability to detect fine detail, have many, densely packed mechanoreceptors with small receptive fields, while the back and legs, for example, have fewer receptors with large receptive fields. Receptors with large receptive fields usually have a "hot spot", an area within the receptive field (usually in the center, directly over the receptor) where stimulation produces the most intense response.
Visual system
In the visual system, receptive fields are volumes in
visual space . For example, the receptive field of a singlephotoreceptor is a cone-shaped volume comprising all the visual directions in which light will alter the firing of that cell. Itsapex is located in the center of the lens and its base essentially atinfinity in visual space. Traditionally, visual receptive fields were portrayed in two dimensions (e.g., as circles, squares, or rectangles), but these are simply slices, cut along the screen on which the researcher presented the stimulus, of the volume of space to which a particular cell will respond. In the case ofbinocular neurons in thevisual cortex , receptive fields do not extend tooptical infinity . Instead, they are restricted to a certain interval of distance from the animal, or from where the eyes are fixating (seePanum's area ).The receptive field is often identified as the region of the
retina where the action oflight alters the firing of the neuron. In retinal ganglion cells (see below), this area of the retina would encompass all thephotoreceptor s, all the rods and cones from oneeye that are connected to this particular ganglion cell viabipolar cell s,horizontal cell s, andamacrine cell s. In binocular neurons in the visual cortex, it is necessary to specify the corresponding area in both retinas (one in each eye). Although these can be mapped separately in each retina by shutting one or the other eye, the full influence on the neuron's firing is revealed only when both eyes are open.Hubel and Wiesel (e.g., Hubel, 1963) advanced the theory that receptive fields of cells at one level of the visual system are formed from input by cells at a lower level of the visual system. In this way, small, simple receptive fields could be combined to form large, complex receptive fields. Later theorists elaborated this simple, hierarchical arrangement by allowing cells at one level of the visual system to be influenced by feedback from higher levels.
Receptive fields have been mapped for all levels of the visual system from photoreceptors, to retinal ganglion cells, to lateral geniculate nucleus cells, to visual cortex cells, to extrastriate cortical cells.
Retinal ganglion cells
The organization of ganglion cells' receptive fields, composed of inputs from many rods and cones, provides a way of detecting contrast, and is used for detecting objects' edges. Each receptive field is arranged into a central disk, the "centre", and a concentric ring, the "surround", each region responding oppositely to light. For example, light in the centre might increase the firing of a particular ganglion cell, whereas light in the surround would decrease the firing of that cell.
There are two types of bipolar cells: "on-center" and "off-center". An on-center cell is stimulated when the center of its receptive field is exposed to light, and is inhibited when the surround is exposed to light. Off-center cells have just the opposite reaction. Stimulation of the center of an on-center cell's receptive field produces "
depolarization " and an increase in the firing of the ganglion cell, stimulation of thesurround produces a "Hyperpolarization " and a decrease in the firing of the cell, and stimulation of both the center and surround produces only a mild response (due to mutual inhibition of center and surround). An off-center cell is stimulated by activation of the surround and inhibited by stimulation of the center (see figure).Photoreceptors that are part of the receptive fields of more than one ganglion cell are able to excite or inhibit
postsynaptic neuron s because they release theneurotransmitter glutamate at theirsynapse s, which can act to depolarize or to hyperpolarize a cell, depending on theion channel s it opens.The center-surround receptive field organization allows ganglion cells to transmit information not merely about whether photoreceptor cells are firing (Photoreceptors do not actually fire action potentials), but also about the differences in firing rates of cells in the center and surround. This allows them to transmit information about contrast. The size of the receptive field governs the
spatial frequency of the information: small receptive fields are stimulated by high spatial frequencies, fine detail; large receptive fields are stimulated by low spatial frequencies, coarse detail. Retinal ganglion cell receptive fields convey information about discontinuities in the distribution of light falling on the retina; these often specify the edges of objects.Lateral geniculate nucleus
Further along in the visual system, groups of ganglion cells form the receptive fields of cells in the
lateral geniculate nucleus . Receptive fields are similar to those of ganglion cells, with an antagonistic center-surround system and cells that are either on- or off center.Visual cortex
Receptive fields of cells in the visual cortex are larger and have more-complex stimulus requirements than retinal ganglion cells or lateral geniculate nucleus cells. Hubel and Wiesel (e.g., Hubel, 1963) classified receptive fields of cells in the visual cortex into
simple cell s,complex cell s, andhypercomplex cell s. Simple cell receptive fields are elongated, for example with an excitatory central oval, and an inhibitory surrounding region, or approximately rectangular, with one long side being excitatory and the other being inhibitory. Images for these receptive fields need to have a particular orientation in order to excite the cell. For complex-cell receptive fields, a correctly oriented bar of light might need to move in a particular direction in order to excite the cell. For hypercomplex receptive fields, the bar might also need to be of a particular length.Extrastriate visual areas
In extrastriate visual areas, cells can have very large receptive fields requiring very complex images to excite the cell. For example in the
inferotemporal cortex , receptive fields cross the midline of visual space and require images such as radial gratings or hands. It is also believed that in thefusiform face area , images of faces excite the cortex more than other images. This property was one of the earliest major results obtained through fMRI (Kanwisher, McDermott and Chun, 1997); the finding was confirmed later at the neuronal level (Tsao, Freiwald, Tootell and Livingstone, 2006). In a similar vein, people have looked for other category-specific areas; some recent research for example suggests theparahippocampal place area might be somewhat specialised for buildings. However, more recent research has suggested that the fusiform face area is specialised not just for faces, but also for any discrete, within-category discrimination.References
*Hubel, D. H. (1963). The visual cortex of the brain. "Scientific American, 209"(5), 54-62.
*Kandel E.R., Schwartz, J.H., Jessell, T.M. (2000). "Principles of Neural Science", 4th ed., pp.515-520. McGraw-Hill, New York.
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.