Nicholas Spanos

Nicholas Spanos
Hypnosis
Applications

Hypnotherapy
Stage hypnosis
Self-hypnosis

Origins

Animal magnetism
Franz Mesmer
History of hypnosis
James Braid

Key figures

Marques of Puységur
James Esdaile
John Elliotson
Jean-Martin Charcot
Ambroise-Auguste Liébeault
Hippolyte Bernheim
Pierre Janet
Sigmund Freud
Émile Coué
Morton Prince
Clark L. Hull
Andrew Salter
Theodore R. Sarbin
Milton H. Erickson
Stephen Brooks
Dave Elman
Ernest Hilgard
Martin Theodore Orne
André Muller Weitzenhoffer
Theodore Xenophon Barber
Nicholas Spanos
Irving Kirsch

Related topics

Hypnotic susceptibility
Suggestion
Post-hypnotic suggestion
Age regression in therapy
Neuro-linguistic programming
Hypnotherapy in the UK

view · talk

Nicholas P. Spanos (1942–June 6, 1994), PhD, was Professor of Psychology and Director of the Laboratory for Experimental Hypnosis at Carleton University from 1975 to his death in a single engine plane crash in 1994.[1]

Contents

Biography

He was an American by birth and received his B.A. and Ph.D. degrees from Boston University. He practiced at Medfield State Hospital and with Boston Psychological Associates before joining the Department of Psychology at Carleton in 1975. He wrote 183 journal articles and 19 chapters for textbooks during his career there. He also wrote for Skeptical Inquirer.[1]

Hypnosis

Nonstate position

Nicholas (Nick) Spanos hypothesized that the behaviors and experiences associated with hypnosis are acted out in accordance with the social context and expectations of the hypnotist and the setting by the person undergoing hypnosis even though they may be sometimes experienced as involuntary. He argued persistently and demonstrated in over 250 experimental studies that hypnotic acts are strongly influenced by the definition of the contexts in which they occur as well as by the cognitive interpretation of the person hypnotized. Spanos argued against Hilgard’s (and others') belief that hypnosis is an altered state of consciousness or a "special" or dissociated state of consciousness. Spanos worked for almost thirty years on this theory, first at the Medfield Foundation with Theodore X. Barber, John Chaves and others, and later at Carleton University in Canada.[1] He argued that many of the actions performed under hypnosis can be simply explained by reference to social psychological and cognitive hypotheses (Spanos, 1996).

Spanos alleged that there are two reasons why people misconstrue their state of consciousness as hypnosis. One being that people believe their behavior is caused by an external source instead of the self. The second is related to the way hypnotic rituals are performed. The hypnotist says certain things which are first interpreted as voluntary and then later on in the procedure as involuntary. An example being “relax the muscles in your legs” and then later “your legs feel limp and heavy.”

Spanos argued that the hypnotist asks each person two connected requests. The first directly asking the subject to do something and the second being for the subject to infer the request as an involuntary one. Some hypnosis participants follow the first request and realize they are performing the task voluntarily while others do not respond at all. Still others follow both requests and therefore deemed great hypnosis subjects.

Using another study, Spanos demonstrates that how people control their hypnotic experience by acting how they believe they are supposed to act during a hypnosis session. The study was performed on two groups of people. One group was given a lecture which included a segment on how arm rigidity was spontaneous during hypnosis and the second group did not. When both groups underwent hypnosis the group who was informed of the arm rigidity demonstrated arm rigidity during the session. A second study used by Spanos involved evaluating the analgesia effect in hypnotic and non-hypnotic individuals. The study performed the experiment on two groups of people and the only difference between the groups is that one group was told they were going to be hypnotized. Each participant was asked to put his or her hand in a bucket of ice and hold it there as long as possible. After removing their arm they were asked to rate their perceived pain. Without hypnosis, those individuals who were expecting hypnosis had a much higher pain rating than those who were not expecting. All participants were 'hypnotized' and then were asked to put their arm in the bucket of ice once more. The people who were not expecting hypnosis had about the same rating of perceived pain as their corresponding non-hypnotic trial. The expecting participants had a much lower rating then their corresponding non-hypnotic trial. Spanos claimed that this was due to the subjects wanting to be viewed as a great hypnotic subject. Spanos’ findings were to contribute to the view that the hypnotic state did not exist at all, and that the behaviors exhibited by those individuals are in fact due to their being “highly motivated”.

Selected Bibliography

  • Spanos, N. P. (1996). Multiple Identities & False Memories: A Sociocognitive Perspective. Washington: American Psychological Association.

References

  1. ^ a b c Baker, Robert A. (Fall 1994), "In Memoriam: Nick Spanos", Skeptical Inquirer 18 (5): 459 

Hock R. R. (2005). Forty studies that changed psychology: explorations into history of psychological research. Upper Saddle River: Pearson.


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Nicholas Spanos — Nicholas P. Spanos, né en 1942 et mort en 1994, est un psychologue connu pour ses travaux sur l hypnose. Œuvres (en) avec Theodore Barber et John F. Chaves, Hypnosis, Imagination, and Human Potentialities, New York, Pergamon, 1974 (en) avec… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Nicholas Spano — Nicholas A. Spano Member of the New York Senate from the 35th district In office 1987–2006 Preceded by John E. Flynn Succeeded by Andrea Stewart Cousins Member of the …   Wikipedia

  • Hypnosis — For the states induced by hypnotic drugs, see Sleep and Unconsciousness. Hypnotized redirects here. For other uses, see Hypnotized (disambiguation). Hypnosis Applications Hypnotherapy Stage hypnosis Self hypnosis Origins Animal magnetism Franz… …   Wikipedia

  • Mal des Ardents — Ergotisme Ergotisme Classification et ressources externes CIM 10 T62.2 CIM 9 988.2 DiseasesDB 30715 MeSH …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Mal des ardents — Ergotisme Ergotisme Classification et ressources externes CIM 10 T62.2 CIM 9 988.2 DiseasesDB 30715 MeSH …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Hipnosis — La hipnosis es un estado mental o de un grupo de actitudes generadas a través de un procedimiento llamado inducción hipnótica. Usualmente se compone de una serie de instrucciones y sugerencias preliminares.[1] Dichas sugestiones pueden ser… …   Wikipedia Español

  • Multiple personality controversy — The existence of multiple personalities within an individual personality is diagnosed as dissociative identity disorder (DID) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM). Prior to the publication of the DSM IV , it was… …   Wikipedia

  • Theodore Barber — Theodore Xenophon Barber (* 1927 in Martins Ferry, OH; † 10. September 2005 in Framingham, MA) war ein amerikanischer Psychologe, der durch seine Kritik an der klassischen Hypnose bekannt wurde. Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Leben 2 Veröffentlichungen …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Theodore Barber — Theodore Xenophon Barber, né en 1927 et mort le 10 septembre 2005, est un docteur en psychologie américain connu pour ses travaux sur l hypnose. Œuvres (en) Hypnosis: A Scientific Approach, 1969 (en) avec Nicholas Spanos et John F.… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Гипноз — Сеанс гипноза, картина Рихарда Берга …   Википедия

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”