Cultural cognition

Cultural cognition

The Cultural cognition of risk, sometimes called simply cultural cognition, refers to the hypothesized tendency of persons to form perceptions of risk and related facts that cohere with their self-defining values. Research examining this phenomenon draws on a variety of social science disciplines including psychology, anthropology, political science, sociology, and communications. The stated objectives of this research are both to understand how values shape political conflict over facts (like whether climate change exists, whether gun control increases crime, whether vaccination of school girls for HPV threatens their health) and to promote effective deliberative strategies for resolving such conflicts consistent with sound empirical data.

Contents

Theory and evidence

The cultural cognition hypothesis holds that individuals are motivated by a variety of psychological processes to form beliefs about putatively dangerous activities that match their cultural evaluations of them. Persons who subscribe to relatively individualistic values, for example, tend to value commerce and industry and are inclined to disbelieve that such activities pose serious environmental risks. Persons who subscribe to relatively egalitarian and communitarian values, in contrast, readily credit claims of environmental risks, consistent with their moral suspicion of commerce and industry as sources of inequality and symbols of excessive self-seeking.[1][2]

Scholars have furnished two types of evidence to support the cultural cognition hypothesis. The first consists of general survey data that suggest that individuals’ values more strongly predict their risk perceptions than do other characteristics such as race, gender, economic status, and political orientations.[3][4]

The second type of evidence consists in experiments that identify discrete psychological processes that connect individuals’ values to their beliefs about risk and related facts.[5] Such experiments suggest, for example, that individuals selectively credit or dismiss information in a manner that reinforces beliefs congenial to their values.[6] They also show that individuals tend to be more persuaded by policy experts perceived to hold values similar to theirs than by ones perceived to hold values different from theirs.[7] Such processes, the experiments suggest, often result in divisive forms of cultural conflict over facts, but can also be managed in fashions that reduce such disagreement.[8]

Cultural cognition project at Yale Law School

Funded by governmental and private foundation grants, much of the work on cultural cognition has been performed by an interdisciplinary group of scholars affiliated with the Cultural Cognition Project [1]. There are currently over a dozen project members, from a variety of universities. Two members of the project—Dan Kahan and Douglas Kysar—are Yale Law School faculty, although other members (such as Donald Braman of George Washington University Law School and Geoffrey Cohen of the University of Colorado) were previously affiliated with Yale Law School or Yale University.

Significant findings

"White male effect"

Risk-perception researchers have documented that white males are less concerned with a wide variety of risks than are minorities and females, a phenomenon known as the "white male effect" in the risk-perception literature.[9][10] An article by Kahan, Braman, Gastil, Slovic and Mertz presented evidence that the white male effect is an outgrowth of cultural cognition.[11] The article reports data showing that race and gender differences in risk perception are conditional on cultural values: White males in general appear to be less concerned about risks only because a discrete group of white males who subscribe to hierarchical and individualistic values are extremely skeptical that activities important to their cultural roles (commerce, gun ownership) impose harm on society generally. This finding does not imply, however, that white males or white hierarchical and individualistic males are uniquely prone to form risk perceptions that are congenial to their social values and roles.

Nanotechnology

The Cultural Cognition Project has conducted a series of studies on public perceptions of nanotechnology risks and benefits. Combining survey and experimental methods, the studies present evidence that individuals culturally predisposed to be skeptical of environmental risks are both more likely to seek out information on nanotechnology and more likely to infer from that information that nanotechnology’s benefits will outweigh its risks. Individuals culturally predisposed to credit environmental risks construe that same information, when exposed to it in the lab, as implying that nanotechnology’s risks will predominate.[12] The studies also present evidence that individuals tend to credit expert information on nanotechnology—regardless of its content—based on whether they share the perceived cultural values of the expert communicator.[13] The studies were issued by the Project on Emerging Nanotechnologies at the Woodrow Wilson International Center for Scholars, one of the research sponsors.

"Scientific Consensus"

The same dynamics that motivate individuals of diverse cultural outlooks to form competing perceptions of risks are likely to cause them to form opposing perceptions of "scientific consensus," cultural cognition researchers have concluded.[14] In an experimental study, the researchers found that subjects were substantially more likely to count a scientist (of elite credentials) as an "expert" in his field of study when the scientist was depicted as taking a position consistent with the one associated with the subjects’ cultural predispositions than when that scientist took a contrary position. A related survey showed that members of opposed cultural groups hold highly divergent impressions of what most scientific experts believe on various matters, a finding consistent with the ubiquity of culturally biased recognition of who counts as an "expert." Across a range of diverse risks (including climate change, nuclear waste disposal, and private handgun possession), members of no particular cultural group, the study found, were more likely than any other to hold perceptions of scientific consensus that consistently matched those adopted in "expert consensus reports" issued by the U.S. National Academy of Sciences.

Law

Scholars have also applied the cultural cognition of risk to legal issues. One such study examined how individuals reacted to a videotape of a high-speed police chase. In Scott v. Harris,[15] the U.S. Supreme Court (by a vote of 8-1) had held that “no reasonable jury” could view the tape and fail to find that the driver posed a lethal risk to the public large enough to justify deadly force by the police (namely, ramming the fleeing driver’s vehicle, causing it to crash). The majority of study subjects agreed with the Court, but there were significant divisions along cultural lines.[16] Other studies have found that individuals’ cultural worldviews influence their perceptions of consent in an acquaintance or date rape scenario,[17] and of the imminence of violence and other facts in self-defense cases involving either battered women or interracial confrontations.[18]

Relationship to other risk perception theories

Cultural cognition is a descendant of two other theories of risk perception. The first is the cultural theory of risk associated with anthropologist Mary Douglas and political scientist Aaron Wildavsky.[19] The cultural cognition hypothesis is derived from Douglas and Wildavsky's claim, advanced most notably in their controversial book Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technical and Environmental Dangers (1982), that individuals selectively attend to risks in a manner that expresses and reinforces their preferred way of life.

Cultural cognition researchers, along with other scholars who have investigated Douglas and Wildavsky’s theory empirically,[20][21][22] use attitudinal scales that reflect Douglas’s worldview typology. That typology characterizes worldviews, or preferences about how society should be organized, along two cross-cutting dimensions: “group,” which refers to how individualistic or group-oriented a society should be; and “grid,” which refers to how hierarchical or egalitarian a society should be.[23]

The second theory is the “psychometric paradigm,” to which Paul Slovic, a member of the Cultural Cognition Project, has made significant contributions. The psychometric paradigm links risk perceptions to various cognitive and social mechanisms that generally evade simpler, rational choice models associated with economics.[24][25] Cultural cognition theory posits that these mechanisms mediate between, or connect, individuals’ cultural values to their perceptions of risk and other policy-relevant beliefs.

Combing the cultural theory of risk and the psychometric paradigm, cultural cognition, its exponents claim, remedies difficulties with each.[26] The mechanisms featured in the psychometric paradigm (and in social psychology generally) furnish a cogent explanation of why individuals adopt states of mind that fit and promote the aims of groups, including ones featured in Douglas’s culture theory. They do so, moreover, in a manner that avoids “functionalism,” a criticized form of analysis that identifies group interests, rather than individual ones, as a cause for human action.[27][28] At the same time cultural theory, by asserting the orienting role of values, explains how the mechanisms featured in the psychometric paradigm can result in differences in risk perception among persons who hold different values. The interrelationship between individual values and perceptions of risk also calls into doubt the depiction of risk perceptions deriving from these mechanisms as products of irrationality or cognitive defect.[29]

Criticisms

Cultural cognition has been subjected to criticisms from a variety of sources. Some rational choice economists[30] as well as some psychologists[31] have suggested that the theory (and ones based on the cultural theory of risk generally) explain only a small fraction of the variation in popular risk perceptions. Some adherents to the cultural theory of risk, including Mary Douglas herself,[32] have criticized cultural cognition for a conception of values that is too tightly modeled on American political disputes and that implicitly disparages the “hierarchical” worldview. Finally, some scholars who emphasize elements of the psychometric paradigm suggest that the influence of cultural values on risk perceptions is best understood as simply an additional source of interference with the rational processing of information.[33]

Notes

  1. ^ Kahan, Fixing the Communications Failure.
  2. ^ Kahan, Slovic, Braman & Gastil, Fear of Democracy, pp. 1083-84.
  3. ^ Kahan & Braman, Cultural Cognition and Public Policy, pp. 155-58.
  4. ^ Kahan, Slovic, Braman & Gastil, Fear of Democracy, pp. 1086-87.
  5. ^ Cultural Cognition Project, Second National Risk and Culture Study.
  6. ^ Kahan, Braman, Slovic, Cohen & Gastil, Cultural Cognition of the Risks and Benefits of Nanotechnology.
  7. ^ Kahan, Braman, Cohen, Slovic & Gastil, Who Fears the HPV Vaccine?
  8. ^ Kahan, The Cognitively Illiberal State.
  9. ^ Finucane, Slovic, Mertz & Flynn, Gender, Race, and Perceived Risk: The "White Male Effect"
  10. ^ Palmer, Risk Perception: Another Look at the White Male Effect.
  11. ^ Culture and Identity-Protective Cognition: Explaining the White-Male Effect in Risk Perception.
  12. ^ Kahan, Braman, Slovic, Cohen & Gastil, Cultural Cognition of the Risks and Benefits of Nanotechnology.
  13. ^ Cultural Cognition Project, Biased Assimilation, Polarization, and Cultural Credibility.
  14. ^ Kahan, Jenkins-Smith & Braman, Cultural Cognition of Scientific Consensus.
  15. ^ 550 U.S. 372 (2007).
  16. ^ Kahan, Hoffman & Braman, Whose Eyes Are You Going to Believe?
  17. ^ Kahan, Culture, Cognition, and Consent.
  18. ^ Kahan & Braman, Self-Defensive Cognition of Self-Defense.
  19. ^ Rayner, Cultural Theory and Risk Analysis.
  20. ^ Dake, Orienting Dispositions in the Perception of Risk: An Analysis of Contemporary Worldviews and Cultural Biases.
  21. ^ Jenkins-Smith, Modeling Stigma.
  22. ^ Peters & Slovic The Role of Affect and Worldviews as Orienting Dispositions in the Perception and Acceptance of Nuclear Power.
  23. ^ Rayner, Cultural Theory and Risk Analysis, pp. 87-91.
  24. ^ Slovic, The Perception of Risk.
  25. ^ Kahneman, Slovic & Tversky, Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases.
  26. ^ Kahan, Cultural Cognition as a Conception of the Cultural Theory of Risk.
  27. ^ Boholm, Risk Perception: Critique of Cultural Theory, pp. 68, 79-80.
  28. ^ Kahan & Braman, Cultural Cognition and Public Policy, p. 252.
  29. ^ Kahan, Slovic, Braman & Gastil, Fear of Democracy, pp. 1088-1106.
  30. ^ Fremling & Lott, The Surprising Finding That “Cultural Worldviews” Don't Explain People's Views On Gun Control.
  31. ^ Sjöberg, World Views, Political Attitudes, and Risk Perception.
  32. ^ Douglas, Being Fair to Hierarchists.
  33. ^ Sunstein, Misfearing.

References

Boholm, Å. (1996). Risk perception and social anthropology: critique of cultural theory. Ethnos, 68(2), 159-178.

Cultural Cognition Project (2008). Biased Assimilation, Polarization, and Cultural Credibility: An Experimental Study of Nanotechnology Risk Perceptions. Project on Emerging Nanotechnologies Research Brief No. 3.

Cultural Cognition Project (2007). The Second National Risk and Culture Study.

Dake, K. (1991). Orienting Dispositions in the Perception of Risk: An Analysis of Contemporary Worldviews and Cultural Biases. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22, 61.

DiMaggio, P. (1997). Culture and Cognition. Annual Review of Sociology, 23, 263-287.

Douglas, M. (2003). Being Fair to Hierarchists. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 151(4), 1349-1370.

Douglas, M., & Wildavsky, A. B. (1982). Risk and Culture : An Essay on the Selection of Technical and Environmental Dangers. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Finucane, M., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Flynn, J., & Satterfield, T. A. (2000). Gender, Race, and Perceived Risk: The "White Male" Effect. Health, Risk, and Society, 3(2), 159-172.

Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, Race, and Perception of Environmental Health Risk. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 1101-1108.

Fremling, G. M., & Lott, J. R. (2003). The Surprising Finding That “Cultural Worldviews” Don't Explain People's Views On Gun Control. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 151(4), 1341-1348.

Jenkins-Smith, H. (2001). Modeling Stigma: An Empirical Analysis of Nuclear Waste Images of Nevada. In J. Flynn, P. Slovic & H. Kunreuther (Eds.), Risk, Media, and Stigma: Understanding Public Challenges to Modern Science and Technology (pp. 107-132). London ; Sterling, VA: Earthscan.

Kahan, D. (2007). The Cognitively Illiberal State. Stanford Law Review, 60, 115-154.

Kahan, D. (2008). Cultural Cognition as a Conception of the Cultural Theory of Risk. Cultural Cognition Project Working Paper No. 73.

Kahan, D. (2010). Culture, Cognition, and Consent: Who Perceives What, and Why, in Acquaintance Rape Cases. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 158, 729-812.

Kahan, D. (2010). Fixing the Communications Failure. Nature, 463, 296-297.

Kahan, D., & Braman, D. (2008). The Self-defensive Cognition of Self-defense. American Criminal Law Review, 45(1), 1-65.

Kahan, D., & Braman, D. (2006). Cultural Cognition of Public Policy. Yale Journal of Law and Public Policy, 24, 147-170.

Kahan, D., Braman, D., Gastil, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (2007). Culture and Identity-Protective Cognition: Explaining the White-Male Effect in Risk Perception. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 4(3), 465-505.

Kahan, D., Braman, D., Cohen, G., Slovic, P., & Gastil, J., (in press). Who Fears the HPV Vaccine, Who Doesn't, and Why: An Experimental Study of the Mechanisms of Cultural Cognition. Law and Human Behavior.

Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2010). Cultural Cognition of Scientific Consensus. Journal of Risk Research. On-line advance publication at http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2010.511246.

Kahan, D., Hoffman, D.A. & Braman, D. (2009). Whose Eyes are You Going to Believe? Scott v. Harris and the Perils of Cognitive Illiberalism. Harvard Law Review, 122(3), 837-906.

Kahan, D., Slovic, P., Braman, D., Cohen G., Gastil, J. (2009). Cultural Cognition of the Risks and Benefits of Nanotechnology. Nature Nanotechnology, 4(2), 87-90.

Kahan, D., Slovic, P., Braman, D., & Gastil, J. (2006). Fear of Democracy: A Cultural Critique of Sunstein on Risk. Harvard Law Review, 119, 1071-1109.

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge ; New York: Cambridge University Press.

Palmer, C. (2003). Risk Perception: Another Look at the “White Male Effect.” Health, Risk & Society, 5(1), 71-83.

Peters, E., & Slovic, P. (1996). The Role of Affect and Worldviews as Orienting Dispositions in the Perception and Acceptance of Nuclear Power. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(16), 1427-1453.

Rayner, S. (1992). Cultural Theory and Risk Analysis. In S. Krimsky & D. Golding (Eds.), Social Theories of Risk (pp. pp. 83-115). Westport, Conn.: Praeger.

Sjöberg, L. (1998). World Views, Political Attitudes, and Risk Perception. Risk: Health, Safety and Environment, 9, 137-152.

Slovic, P. (2000). The Perception of Risk. London: Sterling, VA: Earthscan Publications.

Sunstein, C. R. (2006). Misfearing: A Reply. Harvard Law Review, 119(4), 1110-1125.

Further reading

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Cultural Theory of risk — The Cultural Theory of risk, often referred to simply as Cultural Theory (with capital letters; not to be confused with culture theory), consists of a conceptual framework and an associated body of empirical studies that seek to explain societal… …   Wikipedia

  • Cultural neuroscience — is the study of how cultural values, practices and beliefs shape and are shaped by the mind, brain and genes across multiple timescales.[1] The study of cultural neuroscience bridges theory and methods from anthropology, psychology, neuroscience… …   Wikipedia

  • Cultural-historical psychology — (also called the school of Vygotsky, sociocultural psychology, socio historical psychology, activity theory, cultural psychology, cultural historical activity theory, and social development theory) is a theory of psychology founded by Lev… …   Wikipedia

  • Cultural psychology — Psychology …   Wikipedia

  • Cultural universal — Human universals redirects here. For the 1991 anthropology book, see Human Universals. A cultural universal (also called an anthropological universal or human universal), as discussed by George Murdock, Claude Lévi Strauss, Donald Brown and… …   Wikipedia

  • Culture in music cognition — refers to the impact that a person s culture has on their music cognition, including their preferences, emotion recognition, and musical memory. Musical preferences are biased toward culturally familiar musical traditions beginning in infancy,… …   Wikipedia

  • Group cognition — The concept of group cognition Group cognition is a social, largely linguistic phenomenon whereby a small group of people produce a sequence of utterances that performs a cognitive act. That is, if a similar sequence was uttered or thought by an… …   Wikipedia

  • Socially Distributed Cognition — Distributed cognition is a psychological theory developed in the mid 1980s by Edwin Hutchins. Using insights from sociology, cognitive science, and the psychology of Vygotsky (cf. cultural historical psychology) it emphasizes the social aspects… …   Wikipedia

  • Music cognition — is an interdisciplinary approach to understanding the mental processes that support musical behaviors, including perception, comprehension, memory, attention, and performance. Originally arising in fields of psychoacoustics and sensation,… …   Wikipedia

  • Situated cognition — describes a perspective of human cognition that asserts learning happens as human beings interact with the living world. Also referred to as the situativity theory of cognition (Greeno, 1998), it is a theory of thinking as mainly on the fly and… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”