- Trichromacy
Trichromacy is the condition of possessing three independent channels for conveying
color information, derived from the three different cone types. [ [http://tigger.uic.edu/~hilbert/Glossary.html Color Glossary ] ] Organisms with trichromacy are called trichromats.The normal explanation of trichromacy is that the organism's
retina contains three types ofcolor receptors (calledcone cell s in vertebrates) with different absorption spectra. In practice the number of such receptor types may be greater than three, since different types may be active at different light intensities. In vertebrates with three types of cone cells, at low light intensities therod cell s may contribute tocolor vision , giving a small region oftetrachromacy in the color space.Human s and other closely related primates are usually trichromats, as are femaleNew World monkey s of most species, and both male and femalehowler monkey s. Recent research suggests that trichromacy may also be quite general amongmarsupial s. Most other mammals are currently thought to bedichromat s, with only two types of cone (though the possibility of limited trichromacy at low light levels where the rods and cones are both active). Some species ofinsects (such ashoneybee s) are also trichromats, being sensitive toultraviolet , blue and green instead of blue, green and red. [Rowe, Michael H (2002). " [http://physiologyonline.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/17/3/93 Trichromatic color vision in primates] ." "News in Physiological Sciences." 17(3), 93-98.]Trichromatic color vision
Trichromatic color vision is the ability of humans and some other animals to see different
color s, mediated by interactions among three types of color-sensingcone cell s. The "trichromatic color theory" began in the 18th century, when Thomas Young proposed that color vision was a result of three differentphotoreceptors .Hermann von Helmholtz later expanding on Young's ideas using color-matching experiments which showed that people with normal vision needed three wavelengths to create the normal range of colors. Each of the three types of cones in theretina of theeye contains a different type ofphotosensitive pigment , which is composed of atransmembrane protein calledopsin and a light-sensitive molecule called11-cis retinal . Each different pigment is especially sensitive to a certainwavelength oflight (that is, the pigment is most likely to produce a cellular response when it is hit by aphoton with the specific wavelength to which that pigment is most sensitive). The three types of cones are L, M, and S, which have pigments that respond best to light of long (especially 560 nm), medium (530 nm), and short (420 nm) wavelengths respectively.Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM. "Principles of Neural Science ", 4th ed., pp.182-185. McGraw-Hill, New York (2000). ISBN 0-8385-7701-6]Since the likelihood of response of a given cone varies not only with the "wavelength" of the light that hits it but also with its "intensity", the
brain would not be able to discriminate different colors if it had input from only one type of cone. Thus, interactions between at least two types of cone is necessary to produce the ability to perceive color. With at least two types of cones, the brain can compare the signals from each type and determine both the intensity and color of the light. For example, moderate stimulation of a medium-wavelength cone cell could mean that it is being stimulated by very bright red (long-wavelength) light, or by not very intense yellowish-green light. But very bright red light would produce a stronger response from L cones than from M cones, while not very intense yellowish light would produce a stronger response from M cones than from other cones (counterintuitively, a "strong response" here refers to a large hyperpolarization, since rods and cones communicate that they are being stimulated by "not" firing). Thus trichromatic color vision is accomplished by using combinations of cell responses.References
*Arrese, CA; Oddy, AY; Runham, PB; Hart, NS; Shand, J; Hunt, DM (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka ("Setonix brachyurus") and quenda ("Isoodon obesulus")." "Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B." 272(1595), 791-796.
*Calderone, JB; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Spectral properties and retinal distribution of ferret cones." "Visual Neuroscience." 20(1), 11-17.
*Calderone, JB; Reese, BE; Jacobs, GH (2003). "Topography of photoreceptors and retinal ganglion cells in the spotted hyena ("Crocuta crocuta")." "Brain Behavior and Evolution." 62(4), 182-192.External links
* [http://www.straightdope.com/classics/a2_004.html The Straight Dope] : "Are cats and dogs really color-blind? How do they know?"
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.