- Edward C. Tolman
Infobox Scientist
name = Edward Chace Tolman
box_width =
image_width =150px
caption = Edward Chace Tolman
birth_date =14 Apr 1886
birth_place = West Newton,Massachusetts
death_date =19 Nov 1959
death_place =
residence =
citizenship =
nationality = American
ethnicity =
field = psychologist
work_institutions =
alma_mater =
doctoral_advisor =
doctoral_students =
known_for =behavioral psychology
author_abbrev_bot =
author_abbrev_zoo =
influences =
influenced =
prizes =
religion =
footnotes =
Edward Chace Tolman (1886 - 1959) was an American psychologist. He was most famous for his studies onbehavioral psychology .Born in West Newton,
Massachusetts , brother of CalTech physicistRichard Chace Tolman , Edward C. Tolman studied at theMassachusetts Institute of Technology , and received his Ph.D. fromHarvard University in 1915. Most of his career was spent at theUniversity of California, Berkeley (from 1918 to 1954), where he taught psychology.Tolman is best known for his studies of learning in rats using mazes, and he published many experimental articles, of which his paper with Ritchie and Kalish in 1946 was probably the most influential. His major theoretical contributions came in his 1932 book, "Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men", and in a series of papers in the "
Psychological Review ", "The determinants of behavior at a choice point" (1938) and "Cognitive maps in rats and men" (1948), and "Principles of performance" (1955) [Citation
id =PMID :1431737
url= http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1431737
last=Tolman
first=E C
last2=Ritchie
first2=B F
last3=Kalish
first3=D
publication-date=1992 Dec
year=1992
title=Studies in spatial learning. I. Orientation and the short-cut. 1946.
volume=121
issue=4
periodical=Journal of experimental psychology. General
pages=429-34] [Citation
id =PMID :13254969
url= http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13254969
last=TOLMAN
first=E C
publication-date=1955 Sep
year=1955
title=Principles of performance.
volume=62
issue=5
periodical=Psychological review
pages=315-26] [Citation
id =PMID :13149127
url= http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13149127
last=TOLMAN
first=E C
last2=POSTMAN
first2=L
publication-date=1954
year=1954
title=Learning.
volume=5
issue=
periodical=Annual review of psychology
pages=27-56
doi = 10.1146/annurev.ps.05.020154.000331] [Citation
id =PMID :15398592
url= http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15398592
last=TOLMAN
first=E C
last2=GLEITMAN
first2=H
publication-date=1949 Dec
year=1949
title=Studies in learning and motivation; equal reinforcements in both end-boxes; followed by shock in one end-box.
volume=39
issue=6
periodical=Journal of experimental psychology
pages=810-9] [Citation
id =PMID :15391108
url= http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15391108
last=TOLMAN
first=E C
last2=GLEITMAN
first2=H
publication-date=1949 Oct
year=1949
title=Studies in spatial learning; place and response learning under different degrees of motivation.
volume=39
issue=5
periodical=Journal of experimental psychology
pages=653-9] [Citation
id =PMID :18128182
url= http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18128182
last=TOLMAN
first=E C
publication-date=1949 May
year=1949
title=There is more than one kind of learning.
volume=56
issue=3
periodical=Psychological review
pages=144-55] .Although Tolman was firmly behaviorist in his methodology, he was not a radical behaviorist like
B. F. Skinner . As the title of his 1932 book indicates, he wanted to use behavioral methods to gain an understanding of the mental processes of humans and other animals. In his studies of learning in rats, Tolman sought to demonstrate that animals could learn facts about the world that they could subsequently use in a flexible manner, rather than simply learning automatic responses that were triggered off by environmental stimuli. In the language of the time, Tolman was an "S-S" (stimulus-stimulus), non-reinforcement theorist: he drew onGestalt psychology to argue that animals could learn the connections between stimuli and did not need any explicit biologically significant event to make learning occur. This is known aslatent learning . The rival theory, the much more mechanistic "S-R" (stimulus-response) reinforcement-driven view, was taken up byClark L. Hull .A key paper by Tolman, Ritchie and Kalish in 1946 demonstrated that rats that had explored a maze that contained food while they were not hungry were able to run it correctly on the first trial when they entered it having now been made hungry. However, Hull and his followers were able to produce alternative explanations of Tolman's findings, and the debate between S-S and S-R learning theories became increasingly convoluted and sterile. Skinner's iconoclastic paper of 1950, entitled "Are theories of learning necessary?" persuaded many psychologists interested in animal learning that it was more productive to focus on the behavior itself rather than using it to make hypotheses about mental states. The influence of Tolman's ideas declined rapidly in the later 1950s and 1960s. However, his achievements had been considerable. His 1938 and 1955 papers, produced to answer Hull's charge that he left the rat "buried in thought" in the maze, unable to respond, anticipated and prepared the ground for much later work in
cognitive psychology , as psychologists began to discover and apply decision theory - a stream of work that was recognised by the award of aNobel prize toDaniel Kahneman in 2002. And his 1948 paper introduced the concept of acognitive map , which has found extensive application in almost every field of psychology, frequently among scientists who have no idea that they are using ideas first formulated to explain the behavior of rats in mazes.Furthermore, when in the last quarter of the twentieth century animal psychologists took a cue from the success of human cognitive psychology, and began to renew the study of
animal cognition , many of them turned to Tolman's ideas and to his maze techniques. Of the three great figures of animal psychology of the middle twentieth century, Tolman, Hull and Skinner, it can reasonably be claimed that it is Tolman's legacy that is currently the liveliest, certainly in terms of academic research.Tolman was much concerned that psychology should be applied to try and solve human problems, and in addition to his technical publications, he wrote a book called "Drives Toward War". He was one of the senior professors whom the
University of California sought to dismiss in the McCarthyite era of the early 1950s, because he refused to sign a loyalty oath - not because of any lack of felt loyalty to the United States but because it infringed onacademic freedom . Tolman was a leader of the resistance of the oath, and when theRegents of the University of California sought to fire him, he sued. The resulting court case,Tolman v. Underhill , led to the California Supreme Court in 1955 overturning the oath and forcing the reinstatement of all those who had refused to sign it. In 1963, at the insistence of the then President of the University of CaliforniaClark Kerr , the University named its newly constructed Education and Psychology faculty building at Berkeley "Tolman Hall" in his honor; his widow was present at the dedication ceremony. His portrait hangs in the entrance hall of the building.External links
* [http://tip.psychology.org/tolman.html Account of Tolman's "Sign Learning" theory from the Theory Into Practice database, compiled by Greg Kearsley]
* [http://gse.berkeley.edu/admin/publications/tolmanhistory.html History of Tolman Hall]References
*Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193-216.
*Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century.
*Tolman, E. C. (1938). The determinants of behavior at a choice point. Psychological Review, 45, 1-41.
*Tolman, E. C. (1942). Drives towards war. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
*Tolman, E. C. (1948). [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Tolman/Maps/maps.htm Cognitive maps in rats and men.] Psychological Review, 55, 189-208. (Retrieved on 07-02-06)
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.