- Mnemonist
-
The title mnemonist (derived from the term mnemonic) refers to an individual with the ability to remember and recall unusually long lists of data, such as unfamiliar names, lists of numbers, entries in books, etc. Such individuals have also been described as possessing an eidetic memory, although whether such abilities are innate behaviour or somehow learned appears contentious. Individuals famed as mnemonists have become subjects of scientific inquiry.
Contents
Structure of mnemonic skills
While the innateness of mnemonists’ skills is debated, the methods that mnemonists use to memorize are well-documented. Many mnemonists have been studied in psychology labs over the last century, and most have been found to use mnemonic devices. Currently, all memory champions at the World Memory Championships, have said that they use mnemonic strategies, such as the method of loci, to perform their memory feats.
Skilled memory theory was proposed by K. Anders Ericsson and Bill Chase to explain the effectiveness of mnemonic devices in memory expertise. Generally, short-term memory has a capacity of seven items,[1]; however, in order to memorize long strings of unrelated information, this constraint must be overcome. Skilled memory theory involves three steps: meaningful encoding, retrieval structure, and speed-up[2].
Encoding
In encoding, information is encoded in terms of knowledge structures through meaningful associations. This may initially involve breaking down long lists into more manageable chunks that fall within the capacity of short term memory. Verbal reports of memory experts show a consistent grouping of three or four. A digit sequence 1-9-4-5, for example, can then be remembered as “the year World War Two ended.” Luria’s Solomon Shereshevsky used his synesthesia to associate numbers and words as visual images or colors to encode the information presented to him[3]. Other subjects studied have used previous knowledge such as racing times or historical information[4] to encode new information. This is supported by studies that have shown that previous knowledge about a subject will increase one’s ability to remember it. Chess experts, for example, can memorize more pieces of a chess game in progress than a novice chess player[2]. However, while there is some correlation between memory expertise and general intelligence, as measured by either IQ or the general intelligence factor, the two are by no means identical. Many memory experts have been shown to be average to above-average by these two measures, but not exceptional [5]
Retrieval
The next step is to create a retrieval structure by which the associations can be recalled. It serves the function of storing retrieval cues without having to use short term memory. It is used to preserve the order of items to be remembered. Verbal reports of memory experts show two prominent methods of retrieving information: hierarchical nodes and the method of loci. Retrieval structures are hierarchically organized and can be thought of as nodes that are activated when information is retrieved. Verbal reports have shown that memory experts have different retrieval structures. One expert clustered digits into groups, groups into supergroups, and supergroups into clusters of supergroups. However, by far the most common method of retrieval structure is the method of loci [6].
Method of Loci
Main article: Method of LociThe method of loci is “the use of an orderly arrangement of locations into which one could place the images of things or people that are to be remembered.”[7] The encoding process happens in three step. First, an architectural area, such as the houses on a street, must be memorized. Second, each item to be remembered must be associated with a separate image. Finally, this set of images can be distributed in a “locus,” or place within the architectural area in a pre-determined order. Then, as one tries to recall the information, the mnemonists simply has to “walk” down the street, see each symbol, and recall the associated information. An example of mnemonists who used this is Solomon Shereshevsky; he would use Gorky Street, a street he lived on during his life. When he read, each word would form a graphic image. He would then place this image in a place along the street; later, when he needed to recall the information, he would simply “stroll” down the street again to recall the necessary information[3]. Neuroimaging studies have shown results that support the method of loci as the retrieval method in world-class memory performers. An fMRI recorded brain activity in memory experts and a control group as they were memorizing selected data. Previous studies have shown that teaching a control group the method of loci leads to changes in brain activation during memorization. Consistent with their use of the method of loci, memory experts had higher activity in the medial parietal cortex, retrospenial cortex, and right posterior hippocampus; these brain areas have been linked to spatial memory and navigation. These differences were observable even when the memory experts were trying to memorize stimuli, such as snowflakes, where they showed no superior ability to the control group[8].
Speed Up
The final step in skilled memory theory is speed up. With practice, encoding and retrieval operations can be sped up dramatically. As a result, storage of information can then be performed within a few seconds. Indeed, one confounding factor in the study of memory is that the subjects often improve from day-to-day as they are tested over and over.
Innate vs Learned
The innateness of expert performance in the memory field has been studied thoroughly by many scientists; it is a matter which has still not been definitively resolved.
Evidence for Memory Expertise as a Learned Skill
Much evidence exists which points towards memory expertise as a learned skill which can only be learned through hours of deliberate practice. Anecdotally, the performers in the World Memory Championships all deny any ability of a photographic memory; rather, these experts have averaged 10 years practicing their encoding strategies[6]. Another piece of evidence which points away from an innate superiority of memory is the specificity of memory expertise in memorists. For example, though memory experts have an exceptional ability to remember digits, their ability to remember unrelated items which are more difficult to encode, such as symbols or snowflakes, is the same as that of an average person. The same holds true for memory experts in other fields: studies of mental calculators and chess experts show the same specificity for superior memory[8][2][6]. In some cases, other types of memory, such as visual memory for faces, may even be impaired[9]. Another piece of evidence of memory expertise as a learned ability is the fact that dedicated individuals can make exceptional memory gains when exposed to mnemonics and given a chance to practice. One subject, SF, a college student of average intelligence, was able to attain world-class memory performance after hundreds of hours of practice over two years. His memory, in fact, improved over 70 standard deviations and his digit span, or memory span for digits, grew to 80 digits, which was higher than the digit span for all memory experts previously recorded [2]. Similarly, adults of average intelligence taught encoding strategies also show large gains in memory performance. Finally, neuroimaging studies performed on memory experts and compared to a control group have found no systematic anatomical differences in the brain between memory experts and a control group[8]. While it is true that there are activation differences between the brains of memory experts and a control group, these are due to the use of spatial techniques to form retrieval structures, not any structural differences.
Evidence for Memory Expertise as an Innate Ability
Much of the evidence for innate superiority of memory is anecdotal and is therefore rejected by scientists who have moved toward accepting only reproducible studies as evidence for elite performance. There have been exceptions, however, that do not fit skilled memory theory as proposed by Chase and Ericsson. Synesthetes, for example, show a memory advantage for material that induces their synesthesia over a control group. This advantage tends to be in retention of new information rather than learning. However, synesthetes are likely to have some brain differences which give them an innate advantage when it comes to memory [10]. Another group which may have some innate memory advantage are autistic savants. Unfortunately, many savants who have performed memory feats, such as Kim Peek and Daniel Tammet, have not been studied in a lab; they do claim to not need to use encoding strategies. A recent imaging study of savants found that there are activation differences between savants and typically developing individuals; these cannot be explained by the method of loci as mnemonic savants do not tend to use encoding strategies for their memory. Savants activated the right inferior occipital areas of their brain, whereas control participants activated the left parietal region which is generally associated with attentional processes[11].
Famous Mnemonists
- Creighton Carvello
- Ben Pridmore
- Harry Lorayne
- Dominic O'Brien
- Rajan Mahadevan
- Kim Peek, the real-life inspiration for the character of Raymond in the film Rain Man
- Shass Pollak
- S.V. Shereshevskii, from AR Luria's The Mind of a Mnemonist
- Daniel Tammet, autistic savant. Tammet memorised and recited pi to 22,514 places in five hours to raise money for the National Society for Epilepsy on Pi Day, 2004.
- Ed Cooke
- Wang Feng
- Johannes Mallow
- Brad Williams
- Ramón Campayo
- Srivatsan Rajagopalan
- Shraman N L
See also
References
- ^ Miller, G. A. (1956). "The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information". Psychological Review 63 (2): 343–355
- ^ a b c d Chase, W. G., and Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower, ed., The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 16, pp. 1–58.
- ^ a b Luria, A. (1987). The Mind of Mnemonist. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
- ^ Ericsson, K., Delaney, P., & Weaver, G. (2004). Uncovering the structure of a memorist’s superior “basic” memory capacity. Cognitive Psychology, 49(3), 191-237.
- ^ Conway, A.R.A. Working Memory Capacity and its relation to general intelligence (2003). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (12), pp.547-552.
- ^ a b c Ericsson, K. (2003). Exceptional Memorizers: made, not born. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(6), 233-235.
- ^ Bower, G. (1970) Analysis of a Mnemonic Device. American Scientist. Vol. 58. 496-510.
- ^ a b c Maguire, E. (2003). Routes to remembering: the brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 90-95.
- ^ Raz A. A slice of pi: an exploratory neuroimaging study of digit encoding and retrieval in a superior memorist. Neurocase. 2009 Oct;15(5):361-72.
- ^ Yaro, C., & Ward, J. (2007). Searching for Shereshevskii: What is superior about the memory of synaesthetes?. Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 60(5), 681-695.
- ^ Neumann, N. (2010). The Mind of the Mnemonists: An MEG and Neuropsychological Study of Autistic Memory Savants. Behavioural Brain Research, 215(1), 114-121.
Categories:
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.