Bumblebee communication

Bumblebee communication
Two bumblebees foraging on a flower, taken at Bariloche, Argentina

Bumblebees (Bombus spp.), like the honeybee (Apis spp.) collect nectar and pollen from flowers and store them for food. Many individuals must be recruited to forage for food to provide the hive. Some bee species have highly developed ways of communicating with each other about the location and quality of food resources ranging from physical to chemical displays. Honey bees are known for their specialized dances, such as the waggle dance which recruit other bees to the precise location of the food source [1]. Bumblebees are not capable of transmitting this type of detailed information. Instead, the nest serves as a hub where bees receive information about the foraging bouts of her conspecifics [2]. Differences between the communication methods of honeybees and bumblebees are mainly due to differences in colony size and nest structure [1]. Bumblebees are distinct from honeybees because they lack receiver bees (bees in the nest which receive pollen and nectar from incoming foragers during unloading) and are not capable of trophallaxis (the transfer of nectar from one bee to another). They deposit collected nectar directly into the honey pots and don’t share information of the quality of the resource with other bees through nectar transfer [3]. Another bee may sample the nectar brought into the nest, and if the colony is in need of food or the nectar is high quality she will likely go out foraging herself [3]. Other means of alerting passive bees to a potentially rewarding resource include releasing pheromone signals and increasing physical activity [3]. For information on communication methods in Honey Bees see Bee learning and communication.

Contents

Pioneer Research

Anna Dornhaus (University of Arizona) and Lars Chittka (Queen Mary University of London) began publishing their findings on the communication systems of bumblebees in 1999 when Ms. Dornhaus was an MSc. student in the Chittka lab [4]. Their next major paper on the food alert system in bumblebees came in 2001 probing the potential evolutionary mechanisms of forager recruitment in a bumblebee nest [5]. In 2004 they, along with A. Brockmann (University of Wuzburg) published another paper regarding the pheromone released by bees upon returning to the nest and the type of information it provides other bees [6].

Monitoring Honey pots

Bumblebee nest with full visible honey pots

A bumblebee nest differs in size and structure from that of a honeybee. Bumblebee nests are smaller and contain far fewer individuals which is mostly due to differences in the method of colony reproduction [1]. Honeybee colonies can contain up to 20000 individuals whereas bumblebee colonies usually only contain a few hundred [1]. This means the number of bees available for foraging is low and resources must be allocated accordingly. Assessing the level of food stores is not seen in honeybees, likely because the large colonies make such assessment inefficient [3]. Bumblebees only store enough honey for a few days [7]. By monitoring the levels of the honey pots a bumblebee colony can either up or down regulate the number of bees out foraging[3]. Lab experiments by Anna Dornhaus and Lars Chittka in 2005 showed evidence of this up or down regulation by monitoring the activity level of the hive after the addition of ‘nectar ’ to the honey pots. Hive activity increased when high quality nectar was injected into the honey pots, provided the wells weren’t already full. When the honey pots were full, there was no significant change in activity regardless of whether the nectar imported was from a high or low quality source[3]. They hypothesized that either the foraging bee does not signal the nest or the nest bees ignore the signal because the demand for food is low. The tropical bumblebee Bombus transversalis has also been shown to respond to honey pot levels in a similar way [8].

Excited Runs

Honeybees have very controlled patterns of movement, such as the waggle or tremble dance which serve to deliver specific coordinates of fruitful sources to potential foragers. Bumblebee movement is comparatively random and does not supply coordinates to other bees [5]. Other experiments by Dornhaus and Chittka (2001) showed increased movement of successful foraging bees upon returning to the nest. Successful bees ran faster and longer compared to unsuccessful bees. A bee may spend several minutes running around the nest before flying out again [5]. As the bee runs, it has been hypothesized that the bee may also offer a form of communication based on the buzzing sounds made from her wings [4]. These ‘excited’ runs serve in part to rouse other bees into foraging [5].

Pheromone distribution

Bumblebees produce a signalling pheromone from tergal glands located on their dorsal abdomen as discovered in experiments performed by A. Dornhaus, A. Brockmann and L. Chittka in 2003. They monitored the activity of bee colonies after exposure to products from several glands located along the bee’s body. The only one yielding significant changes in activity level came from the tergites VI and VII [6].This is similar to a pheromone produced from the Nasanov gland in honeybees, but differs in the active compound [6]. It has been suggested that this pheromone may facilitate learning of floral scents, since its release is coupled with the import of the floral scent from the nectar collected by the successful forager [6]. Experiments by Molet, Chittka and Raine in 2009 showed that bumblebees may be able to learn floral scents associated with rewarding flowers better if the particular scent is found in nectar deposited in the honeypots [9].

References

  1. ^ a b c d Winston ML, 1987. The biology of the honey bee. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  2. ^ Dornhaus, A. and Chittka, L. 2004. Information flow and regulation of foraging activity in bumble bees. Apidologie 35: 183-192.
  3. ^ a b c d e f Dornhaus A and Chittka L. 2005. Bumble bees (Bombus terrestris) store both food and information in honeypots. Behavioural Ecology. 16:661-666.
  4. ^ a b "Anna's past and current research projects." Anna's Homepage. N.p., n.d. Web. 10 Apr. 2011. http://www.dornhaus.de/www.anna/aprojects.html
  5. ^ a b c d Dornhaus A, Chittka L, 2001. Food alert in bumblebees, Bombus terrestris: possible mechanisms and evolutionary implications. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:570–576.
  6. ^ a b c d Dornhaus A, Brockmann A, Chittka L, 2003. Bumblebees alert to food with pheromone from tergal glands. J Comp Phys A 189:47–51.
  7. ^ Heinrich B, 1979. Bumblebee economics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press
  8. ^ Dornhaus A, Cameron S, 2003. A scientific note on food alert in Bombus transversalis. Apidologie 34:87–88.
  9. ^ Molet M, Chittka L and Raine NE. 2009. How floral odours are learned inside the bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) nest. Naturwissenschaften 96:213–219

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Bumblebee (Transformers) — Bumblebee is the name of several fictional characters from the various Transformers universes. Transformers: Generation 1 Transformers character name = Bumblebee japanname = Bumble caption = affiliation = Autobot subgroup = Mini bots,… …   Wikipedia

  • Bumble bee — For other uses, see Bumblebee (disambiguation) and Bombus (disambiguation). Bombus Bombus terrestris Scientific classification …   Wikipedia

  • Transformers Decepticons — is a video game based on fictional alien robots, Transformers. This version follows an alternate plot of the movie.Characters(In order of appearance) * Decepticon Create A Bot (voiced by Steven Jay Blum): Commonly referred to as Starscream s Pet… …   Wikipedia

  • hymenopteran — /huy meuh nop teuhr euhn/, adj. 1. hymenopterous. n. 2. Also, hymenopter. a hymenopterous insect. [1875 80; HYMENOPTER + AN] * * * ▪ insect Introduction   any member of the third largest and perhaps the most beneficial to humans of all insect… …   Universalium

  • List of characters in Transformers (film series) — The following is a list of characters featured in the Transformers film series, directed by Michael Bay and distributed by Paramount Pictures. Contents 1 Humans 1.1 Sam Witwicky 1.2 Mikaela Banes 1.3 …   Wikipedia

  • List of Autobots — Autobot Insignia This is a list of known Autobots from the Transformers fictional universe and toy line. The alternate modes of Autobots are usually cars, trucks and various other ground based civilian vehicles. Contents 1 The Transformers… …   Wikipedia

  • Autobot — Autobots Autobot insignia Founder Primus, Dynasty of Primes Current leader Optimus Prime, Rodimus Prime, Ultra Magnus Home world Cybertron, Earth …   Wikipedia

  • Constructicons — Scavenger, Bonecrusher, Longhaul, Scrapper Mixmaster five of the original six Constructicons (Constructicon Hook not shown) The Constructicons are a group of fictional characters from the various Transformers continuities. Although they vary from …   Wikipedia

  • Transformers: Dark of the Moon — This article is about the film. For the related toy line, see Transformers: Dark of the Moon (toy line). For the video game, see Transformers: Dark of the Moon (video game). Transformers: Dark of the Moon …   Wikipedia

  • Insect wing — Original veins and wing posture of a dragonfly. Hoverflies hovering to mate …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”