Challenge Point Framework

Challenge Point Framework

The Challenge Point Framework, created by Mark A. Guadagnoli and Timothy D. Lee (2004), provides a theoretical basis to conceptualize the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. This framework relates practice variables to the skill level of the individual, task difficulty, and information theory concepts. The fundamental idea is that “motor tasks represent different challenges for performers of different abilities” (Guadagnoli and Lee 2004, p212). Any task will present the individual with a certain degree of challenge. However, the learning potential from this task difficulty level will differ based on the:

  1. skill level of the performer
  2. task complexity
  3. task environment

Importantly, though increases in task difficulty may increase learning potential, increased task difficulty is also expected to decrease performance. Thus, an optimal challenge point exists when learning is maximized and detriment to performance in practice is minimized.

Contents

Importance and Applications

Practice has been proposed as the most important factor for the “relatively permanent” improvement in the ability to perform motor skills (Adams 1964; Annett 1969; Fitts 1964; Magill 2001; Marteniuk 1976; Newell 1981; Salmoni et al. 1984; Schmidt and Lee 1999; Guadagnoli and Lee 2004). With all other variables held constant, skill increases with practice (Guadagnoli and Lee 2004). However, time devoted to practice can be made more efficient by careful consideration of practice conditions. The challenge point framework presents a theoretical perspective to consider the roles of the level of the performer, the complexity of the task and the training environment in regulating the learning potential during practice. Adjustment of these components to enhance motor learning can be applied to variety of contexts, including rehabilitation (Descarreaux et al. 2010; Onla-or & Winstein 2008) and simulation-based health professions education (Gofton 2006).

History

The challenge point framework involves concepts generated through various lines of research including information theory, communications theory, and information processing (Lintern and Gopher 1978; Martenuik 1976; K.M. Newell et al. 1991; Wulf and Shea 2002). Specific notions borrowed from prior research important to understanding the theoretical framework include:

  1. Learning is a problem-solving process in which the goal of an action represents the problem to be solved and the evolution of a movement configuration represents the performer’s attempt to solve the problem (Miller et al. 1960 as cited by Guadagnoli and Lee 2004).
  2. Sources of information available during and after each attempt to solve a problem are remembered and form the basis for learning, which is defined as a relatively permanent improvement in skill that results from practice (Guthrie 1952 as cited by Guadagnoli and Lee 2004).
  3. Two sources of information are critical for learning:
    1. An action plan is a construct that invokes intention and ultimately results in a specific movement configuration on a given performance (Miller et al. 1960 as cited by Guadagnoli and Lee 2004). See motor control.
    2. Feedback may be inherent to the individual (e.g. vision) or available via external, augmented sources (e.g. verbal instruction).
  4. Information is transmitted only when uncertainty is reduced (Shannon and Weaver 1949; Fitts 1954; Fitts and Posner 1967; Legge and Barber 1967; Martenuik 1976; Miller 1956 as cited by Guadagnoli and Lee 2004).

Components of the Framework

It follows from the description of the challenge point framework that:

  • It is impossible to learn without information
  • Learning is impaired by presentation of insufficient or excessive amounts of information
  • Learning is facilitated by an optimal amount of information, which depends on individual skill and task difficulty

Information Available and Task Difficulty

Learning is fundamentally a problem-solving process. It has been proposed that with practice, there is reduced information available to the participant because better expectations are formed (i.e. practice = redundancy, therefore less uncertainty; Marteniuk 1976). However, increasing functional task difficulty results in less certainty about the predicted success of the action plan and the nature of the feedback. At low levels of functional difficulty, the potential available information is low for performers in all skill levels. As functional task difficulty increases, the potential information available increases exponentially for beginners and less rapidly for intermediate and skilled performers. For experts, the potential information available increases only at the highest levels of functional task difficulty.

Task Difficulty and Skill

Task difficulty has received considerable attention in prior research (Fleighman and Quaintance 1984; Gentile 1998). Important to the challenge point framework, task difficulty is not explicitly defined. Alternately, two broad categories can encompass these elements:

  • Nominal task difficulty
    • Difficulty due to the characteristics of the task only, reflecting a constant amount of task difficulty (e.g. the target of a throw being near versus far); includes perceptual and motor performance requirements (Swinnen et al. 1992).
  • Functional task difficulty
    • Difficulty due to the person performing the task and the environment (e.g. two individuals, a major league pitcher and an inexperienced ball thrower, are asked to throw a baseball as fast as they can to first base on two days, one on a sunny day and the other on a windy day).

Performance of a task with low nominal difficulty will be expected to be high in all groups of performers (i.e. all skill levels). However, beginner performance will be expected to decline rapidly as nominal difficulty increases, whereas intermediate and skilled performance will decline less rapidly, and expert performance is expected to decline only at the highest nominal difficulty levels.

Although the “Expert” skill level is useful to explain this framework, one may argue that experts should have a high level of predicted success for all nominal task difficulties. It is possible that once expertise is attained, these individuals are able to predict the outcome of the ongoing task and modify ongoing processes in order to reach a suitable outcome (e.g. surgeons).

Optimal Challenge Points

The optimal challenge point represents the degree of functional task difficulty an individual of a specific skill level would need to optimize learning (Guadagnoli and Lee 2004). However, this learning depends on the amount of interpretable information. Therefore, although increases in task difficulty may increase learning potential, only so much is interpretable, and task performance is expected to decrease. Thus, an optimal challenge point exists when learning is maximized and detriment to performance in practice is minimized. With increased practice it is assumed that one’s information-processing capabilities will increase (Marteniuk 1976). Therefore, the optimal challenge point will change as the individual’s ability to use information changes, requiring further changes in functional difficulties in task to facilitate learning (Guadagnoli and Lee 2004).

Practice Variables and Framework Predictions

Contextual Interference (CI) and Action Planning

Predictions from the challenge point framework with respect to CI (refer to motor learning; Guadagnoli and Lee 2004, p 219):

  1. “For tasks with differing levels of nominal difficulty, the advantage of random practice (vs. blocked practice) for learning will be largest for tasks of lowest nominal difficulty and smallest for tasks of highest nominal difficulty”.
  2. “For individuals with differing skill levels, low levels of CI will be better for beginning skill levels and higher levels of CI will be better for more highly skilled individuals”.

Knowledge of Results (KR) and Feedback Information

Predictions from the challenge point framework with respect to KR (refer to motor learning; Guadagnoli and Lee 2004, p221):

  1. “For tasks of high nominal difficulty, more frequent or immediate presentation of KR, or both, will yield the largest learning effect. For tasks of low nominal difficulty, less frequent or immediate presentation of KR, or both, will yield the largest learning effect”.
  2. “For tasks about which multiple sources of augmented information can be provided, the schedule of presenting the information will influence learning. For tasks of low nominal difficulty, a random schedule of augmented feedback presentation will facilitate learning as compared with a blocked presentation. For tasks high in nominal difficulty, a blocked presentation will produce better learning than a random schedule”.

References

  1. Adams J.A. (1964). Motor skills. Annual review of psychology, 15, 181-202.
  2. Annett J. (1969). Feedback and human behaviour. Middlesex, England: Penguin.
  3. Descarreaux M., Passmore S. R., and Cantin V. (2010). Head movement kinematics during rapid aiming task performance in healthy and neck-pain participants: The importance of optimal task difficulty. Manual Therapy, 15(5), 445-450.
  4. Fitts P.M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391.
  5. Fitts P.M. (1964). Perceptual-motor skills learning. In A.W. Melton (Ed.), Categories of human learning (pp. 243–285). New York: Academic Press.
  6. Fitts P.M., and Posner M.I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
  7. Fleishman E.A., and Quaintance M.K. (Eds.) (1984). Taxonomies of human performance: The description of human tasks. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
  8. Gentile A.M. (1998). Implicit and explicit processes during acquisition of functional skills. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 5, 7–16.
  9. Gofton W. (2006). Factors in optimizing the learning environment for surgical training. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, (449), 100-107.
  10. Guadagnoli T.D. and Lee T.D. (2004) Challenge point: A framework for conceptualizing the effect of various practice conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behaviour 36(2): 212-224.
  11. Guthrie E.R. (1952). The psychology of learning. New York: Harper & Row.
  12. Legge D., and Barber P.J. (1976). Information and skill. London: Methuen.
  13. Lintern G. and Gopher D. (1978). Adaptive training of perceptual motor skills: Issues, results and future directions. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 10, 521-551.
  14. Magill R.A. (2001). Motor Learning: Concepts and applications (6th Ed.). Madison, WI: Brown and Benchmark.
  15. Marteniuk R.G. (1976). Information processing in motor skills. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
  16. Miller G.A. (1956). The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
  17. Miller G.A., Galanter E., and Pribram K.H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
  18. Newell K.M. (1981). Skill learning. In D.H. Holding (Ed.), Human skills. New York: Wiley.
  19. Newell K.M., McDonald P.V. and Kugler P.N. (1991). The perceptual-motor workspace and the acquisition of skill. In J. Requin & G.E. Stelmach (Eds.), Tutorials in motor neuroscience (pp. 95–108). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
  20. Onla-or S., and Winstein C.J. (2008). Determining the optimal challenge point for motor skill learning in adults with moderately severe parkinson's disease. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22(4), 385-395.
  21. Salmoni A.W., Schmidt R.A., and Walter C.B. (1984). Knowledge of results and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 355-386.
  22. Schmidt R.A. and Lee T.D. (1999). Motor control and learning: A behavioural emphasis (3rd Ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  23. Shannon C.E., and Weaver W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
  24. Swinnen S.P., Walter C.B., Serrien D.J., and Vandendriessche C. (1992). The effect of movement speed on upper-limb coupling strength. Human Movement Science, 11, 615–636.
  25. Wulf G. and Shea C.H. (2002). Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychonomics Bulletin, 9, 185-211.

External links


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Нужно решить контрольную?

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Seventh Framework Programme — The Seventh Framework Programme for research and technological development (FP7) is the European Union s chief instrument for funding research over the period 2007 to 2013. It bundles all research related EU initiatives together under a common… …   Wikipedia

  • Service-oriented architecture implementation framework — Service oriented architectures (SOA) are based on the notion of software services, which are high level software components that include web services. Implementation of an SOA requires tools as well as run time infrastructure software. This is… …   Wikipedia

  • Micah Challenge UK — Type Anti poverty coalition Location Kennington, London Focus Mobilising Christians against global poverty Website www.micahchallenge.org.uk Micah Challenge UK is a coalition of Christian development agencies, organisat …   Wikipedia

  • Motor learning — is a “relatively permanent” change, resulting from practice or a novel experience, in the capability for responding (Guthrie, 1952). It often involves improving the smoothness and accuracy of movements and is obviously necessary for complicated… …   Wikipedia

  • Motor program — A motor program is an abstract representation of movement that centrally organizes and controls the many degrees of freedom involved in performing an action (Schmidt and Lee, 2005 p. 182). Signals transmitted through efferent and afferent… …   Wikipedia

  • India — /in dee euh/, n. 1. Hindi, Bharat. a republic in S Asia: a union comprising 25 states and 7 union territories; formerly a British colony; gained independence Aug. 15, 1947; became a republic within the Commonwealth of Nations Jan. 26, 1950.… …   Universalium

  • china — /chuy neuh/, n. 1. a translucent ceramic material, biscuit fired at a high temperature, its glaze fired at a low temperature. 2. any porcelain ware. 3. plates, cups, saucers, etc., collectively. 4. figurines made of porcelain or ceramic material …   Universalium

  • China — /chuy neuh/, n. 1. People s Republic of, a country in E Asia. 1,221,591,778; 3,691,502 sq. mi. (9,560,990 sq. km). Cap.: Beijing. 2. Republic of. Also called Nationalist China. a republic consisting mainly of the island of Taiwan off the SE coast …   Universalium

  • United Kingdom — a kingdom in NW Europe, consisting of Great Britain and Northern Ireland: formerly comprising Great Britain and Ireland 1801 1922. 58,610,182; 94,242 sq. mi. (244,100 sq. km). Cap.: London. Abbr.: U.K. Official name, United Kingdom of Great… …   Universalium

  • Europe, history of — Introduction       history of European peoples and cultures from prehistoric times to the present. Europe is a more ambiguous term than most geographic expressions. Its etymology is doubtful, as is the physical extent of the area it designates.… …   Universalium

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”