Andrew N. Meltzoff

Andrew N. Meltzoff

Andrew N. Meltzoff (born February 9, 1950) is an American psychologist and an internationally recognized expert on infant and child development. His discoveries about infant imitation greatly advanced the scientific understanding of early cognition, personality and brain development.

Background

Meltzoff received a B.A. from Harvard University in 1972 and a D.Phil. (Ph.D.) from Oxford University in 1976. A professor of psychology at the University of Washington since 1988, he is currently co-director of the University of Washington Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences. The Institute is an interdisciplinary scientific research center on human learning.

He is married to the internationally recognized speech and hearing scientist and language acquisition researcher Patricia K. Kuhl.

Early research

In 1977, "Science" published the ground-breaking paper "Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates" by Meltzoff, who was still at Oxford, and M. Keith Moore of the University of Washington. [Meltzoff, A.N. and Moore, M.K. (1977). "Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates", "Science", 198, 75-78.] According to the abstract,

Infants between 12 and 21 days of age can imitate both facial and manual gestures; this behavior cannot be explained in terms of either conditioning or innate releasing mechanisms. Such imitation implies that human neonates can equate their own unseen behaviors with gestures they see others perform.

Six infants were each shown three facial gestures and one manual gesture, sequentially. Their responses were videotaped and scored by observers who did not know which gesture the infants had seen. The statistically significant results showed that infants of this young age were able to imitate all four gestures.

The experiment was ground-breaking because it showed infant imitation of adults at a much earlier age than was thought possible. Jean Piaget, for instance, had thought that infants reached the stage of facial imitation at 8 to 12 months. The study also showed early "facial" imitation, something previously thought to be impossible at this young age because of its necessarily crossmodal nature. (Infants can see others' faces but not their own; they can feel their own facial movements, but not those of others.) The findings had implications not only for theoretical psychology, but also for the study of memory, learning, language acquisition, and socialization.

A similar study was later done with a group of 40 infants with a mean age of 72 hours (youngest 42 minutes), with the same results, showing that the intermodal mapping infants displayed was unlikely to be learned. [Meltzoff, A.N. and Moore, M.K. (1983). "Newborn Infants Imitate Adult Facial Gestures", "Child Development", 54, 702-709.] However, later studies have suggested that while neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion is widespread, the findings for the imitation of other gestures at this young age are more mixed. [Anisfeld, M. (1996). "Only Tongue Protrusion Modelling is Matched by Neonates", "Developmental Review", 16, 149-161.] [Jones, S.S. (2007). "Imitation in Infancy: the Development of Mimicry", "Psychological Science", 18, 593-599.]

Methodological innovations

Preverbal infant psychology is notoriously difficult to study. Meltzoff and his colleagues had to develop new techniques for eliciting and interpreting infant responses to stimuli. One method was measuring an infant's visual preference for an object. In one study, infants were allowed to touch but not see a distinctively shaped object. Later they were shown (but could not touch) that object and a different object. The length of time they gazed at each object was measured. Infants looked longer at the object they had previously touched, thus demonstrating an ability to recognize the object with a different sense. [Meltzoff, A.N., & Borton, R.W. (1979). "Intermodal matching by human neonates". "Nature", 282, 403-404.]

In another experiment, babies' sucking on a pacifier was recorded, and a picture was shown to them. When the sucking stopped, the picture disappeared. Babies were found to suck longer when the picture showed a familiar face than when it showed an unfamiliar one.

Later research

Later research has included the investigation of memory; [Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K. (1994). "Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons". "Infant Behavior and Development", 17, 83-99.] communications development in young children with autism; [Toth, Karen, Munson, Jeffrey, Meltzoff, Andrew N. and Dawson, Geraldine (2006). "Early Predictors of Communication Development in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Joint Attention, Imitation, and Toy Play", "J Autism Dev Disord", 36:993–1005] intention; [Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). The ‘like me’ framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. "Acta Psychologica", 124 26–43.] empathy; [Jackson, P.L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A.N., & Decety, J. (2006). "Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain". "Neuropsychologi"a, 44, 752-761.] and gaze-following. [Meltzoff, A.N., & Brooks, R. (2007). "Eyes Wide Shut: The Importance of Eyes in Infant Gaze-Following and Understanding Other Minds", In R. Flom, K. Lee, & D. Muir (Eds.), "Gaze following: Its development and significance" (pp. 217-241). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.]

Theory

Based on his work on imitation, Meltzoff has developed the "like me" hypothesis of infant development. This involves three steps. First, there is an intrinsic, supramodal connection in the infant mind between observed acts and similar executed acts (the correspondence reported in the 1977 and 1983 studies cited above). Secondly, infants experience a regular association between their own acts and their own underlying mental states. This is based on everyday experience. Third, infants project their own internal experiences onto others performing similar acts. As a result, infants begin to acquire an understanding of other minds and their mental states (desires, visual perception and basic emotions, for instance).

This hypothesis suggests that it is imitation that is inborn, and the understanding of other's mental states is a consequence. Other researchers have suggested the opposite, that imitation is a consequence of an understanding of others. But Meltzoff's early imitation studies clearly favor the former possibility. [Meltzoff, Andrew N. (2007). "‘Like me’: a foundation for social cognition", "Developmental Science" 10:1, pp 126–134.]

Honors

*National Institutes of Health MERIT Award
*Outstanding Research Award, Society for Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics
*Kenneth Craig Award in Psychology, Cambridge University, England, 2005
*Norwegian Academy of Science and Letters, Inducted as foreign member

elected works

* [http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/pdf/77Meltzoff_Moore_Science.pdf Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K. (1977). "Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates." "Science", 198, 75-78.]
* [http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/pdf/79Meltzoff_Borton_Nature.pdf Meltzoff, A.N., & Borton, R.W. (1979). Intermodal matching by human neonates. "Nature", 282, 403-404]
*Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). "Words, thoughts, and theories". Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 0262571269
*Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A.N., & Kuhl, P.K. (2000). "The scientist in the crib: What early learning tells us about the mind". New York: HarperCollins. ISBN 0688177883
*Meltzoff, A.N., & Prinz, W. (2002), Eds. "The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases". Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0521806852
*Meltzoff, A.N., & Decety, J. (2003). "What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience." The Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, 358, 491-500.
* [http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/pdf/05Meltzoff_Like%20Me%20Hypoth.pdf Meltzoff, A.N. (2005). "Imitation and other minds: The 'Like Me' hypothesis." In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science (pp. 55-77). Cambridge: MIT Press.]

References

External links

* [http://ilabs.washington.edu/ University of Washington Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences]
* [http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/cv.html Curriculum Vitae]
* [http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/publications.html Publications]


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем решить контрольную работу

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Theory of mind — is the ability to attribute mental states beliefs, intents, desires, pretending, knowledge, etc. to oneself and others and to understand that others have beliefs, desires and intentions that are different from one s own.[1] Though there are… …   Wikipedia

  • List of developmental psychologists — The following is a list of academicians, both past and present, who are widely renowned for their groundbreaking contributions to the field of developmental psychology. A * Mary Ainsworth (1913 1999) B * James Mark Baldwin (1861–1934) * Rachel… …   Wikipedia

  • Philosophical Psychology (journal) — Infobox Journal title = Philosophical Psychology editor = William Bechtel Cees van Leeuwen discipline = Philosophy language = English abbreviation = Phil. Psych. publisher = Routledge country = frequency = 6 issues per year history = 1988 to… …   Wikipedia

  • Liste der Homo-Epitheta — Die Liste der Homo Epitheta (der Beiwörter zu dem lateinischen Wort homo „Mensch“) erfasst alle Ausdrücke, die aus dem lateinischen Substantiv homo („Mensch“) und einem spezifizierenden Adjektiv oder auch Substantiv zusammengesetzt sind. Dazu… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Culture — For other uses, see Culture (disambiguation). Petroglyphs in modern day Gobustan, Azerbaijan, dating back to 10 000 BCE indicating a thriving culture …   Wikipedia

  • Baby Einstein — is a line of multimedia products and toys that specializes in interactive activities for children aged 3 months to 3 years old. Subjects such as classical music, art, and poetry are prominently explored. These products are currently made by a… …   Wikipedia

  • Gedächtnis — Unter Gedächtnis versteht man die Fähigkeit des Nervensystems von Lebewesen, aufgenommene Informationen zu behalten, zu ordnen und wieder abzurufen. Die gespeicherten Informationen sind das Ergebnis von bewussten oder unbewussten Lernprozessen,… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Illustration Magazine — [http://www.illustration magazine.com] is a full color quarterly periodical first published in October 2001 by The Illustrated Press, Inc. Edited, published, and designed by founder Daniel Zimmer, each issue features biographical articles on… …   Wikipedia

  • René Girard — (born December 25, 1923, Avignon, France) is a world renowned French historian, literary critic, and philosopher of social science. His work belongs to the tradition of anthropological philosophy. He is the author of several books (see below),… …   Wikipedia

  • Rene Girard — René Girard Pour les articles homonymes, voir René Girard (homonymie). René Noël Théophile Girard, né à Avignon le 25 décembre 1923, est un philosophe français, membre de l Académie française depuis 2005. Ancien élève de l École des chartes et… …   Wikipédia en Français

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”