- The Spinning Dancer
-
The Spinning Dancer, also known as the silhouette illusion, is a kinetic, bistable optical illusion resembling a pirouetting female dancer. The illusion, created by web designer Nobuyuki Kayahara,[1][2] involves the apparent direction of motion of the figure. Some observers initially see the figure as spinning clockwise (viewed from above) and some counterclockwise.
The illusion derives from the lack of visual cues for depth. For instance, as the dancer's arms move from viewer's left to right, it is possible to view her arms passing between her body and the viewer (that is, in the foreground of the picture, in which case she would be circling counter-clockwise on her right foot) and it is also possible to view her arms as passing behind the dancer's body (that is, in the background of the picture, in which case she is seen circling clockwise on her left foot).
When she is facing to the left or to the right, her profile is unambiguous. That is, her breasts and ponytail clearly define the direction she is facing. However, as she moves away from facing to the left (or from facing to the right), the dancer can be seen (by different viewers, not by a single individual) facing in either of two directions. At first, these two directions are fairly close to each other (both left, say, but one facing slightly forward, the other facing slightly backward) but they become further and further away from each other until we reach a position where her ponytail and breasts are in line with the viewer (so that neither her breasts nor her ponytail are seen so readily). In this position, she could either be facing away from the viewer or toward the viewer, so that the two positions two different viewers could see are 180 degrees apart. Which the viewer sees will depend on which direction of turn the viewer is visualizing.
There are other optical illusions that depend on the same or a similar kind of visual ambiguity. One example is the Necker Cube.
Contents
Psychology of visual perception
The illusion has been incorrectly[3] identified as a scientific personality test that supposedly reveals which hemisphere of the brain is dominant in the observer. Under this unproven interpretation, it has been popularly called the Right Brain–Left Brain test,[4] and was widely circulated on the Internet during late 2008 to early 2009.[citation needed] During this time it was established that the silhouette is more often seen rotating clockwise than counter-clockwise. According to an online survey of over 1600 participants, approximately two thirds of observers initially perceived the silhouette to be rotating clockwise. In addition, observers who initially perceived a clockwise rotation had more difficulty experiencing the alternative.[5]
These results can be explained by a psychological study providing evidence for a viewing-from-above bias that influences observers' perceptions of the silhouette.[6][7] Kayahara's dancer is presented with a camera elevation slightly above the horizontal plane. Consequently, the dancer may also be seen from above or below in addition to spinning clockwise or counter-clockwise, and facing toward or away from the observer. Upon inspection, one may notice that in Kayahara's original illusion, seeing the dancer spin clockwise is paired with constantly holding an elevated viewpoint and seeing the dancer from above. The opposite is also true, an observer maintaining a counter-clockwise percept has assumed a viewpoint below the dancer. If observers report perceiving Kayahara's original silhouette as spinning clockwise more often than counter-clockwise, there are two chief possibilities. They may have a bias to see it spinning clockwise, or they may have a bias to assume a viewpoint from above. To tease these two apart, the researchers created their own versions of Kayahara's silhouette illusion by recreating the dancer and varying the camera elevations. This allowed for clockwise/from-above (like Kayahara's original) and clockwise/from-below pairings. The results indicated that there was no clockwise bias, but rather viewing-from-above bias. Furthermore, this bias was dependent upon camera elevation. In other words, the greater the camera elevation, the more often an observer saw the dancer from above.
Bistable perception
Depending on the perception of the observer, the apparent direction of spin may change any number of times, although some observers may have difficulty perceiving a change in motion at all. One way of changing the direction perceived is to use averted vision and mentally look for an arm going behind instead of in front, then carefully move the eyes back. Some may perceive a change in direction more easily by narrowing visual focus to a specific region of the image, such as the spinning foot or the shadow below the dancer and gradually looking upwards. One can also try to tilt one's head to perceive a change in direction. Another way is to watch the base shadow foot, and perceive it as the toes always pointing away from you and it can help with direction change. You can also close your eyes and try and envision the dancer going in a direction then reopen them and the dancer should change directions. Still another way is to wait for the dancer's legs to cross in the projection and then try to perceive a change in the direction in what follows. You could also try using your peripheral vision to distract the dominant part of the brain, slowly look away from the ballerina and you may begin to see it spin in the other direction. Perhaps the easiest method is to blink rapidly (slightly varying the rate if necessary) until consecutive images are going in the 'new' direction. Then open your eyes and the new rotational direction is maintained. It is even possible to see the illusion in a way that the dancer is not spinning at all, but simply rotating back and forth 180 degrees.
Slightly altered versions of the image exist in which an additional visual cue facilitates the perception of counterclockwise spin and clockwise spin. Looking at one of these can then make the original dancer image above spin in the corresponding direction.
Versions of the image with an additional visual cue Further analysis
References
- ^ Nobuyuki Kayahara's website
- ^ Parker-Pope, Tara (2008-04-28). "The Truth About the Spinning Dancer". Well Blog. The New York Times. http://well.blogs.nytimes.com/2008/04/28/the-truth-about-the-spinning-dancer/. Retrieved 2008-08-07.
- ^ Novella, Steven (2007-10-11). "Left Brain - Right brain and the Spinning Girl". NeuroLogica Blog. http://www.theness.com/neurologicablog/index.php?p=27. Retrieved 2008-08-07.
- ^ "The Right Brain vs Left Brain test". PerthNow. The Sunday Times. 2007-09-26. http://www.news.com.au/perthnow/story/0,21598,22492511-5005375,00.html. Retrieved 2008-08-07.
- ^ "Casual Fridays: TK-421, why can't you spin that woman in reverse?". Cognitive Daily. 2008-10-10. http://scienceblogs.com/cognitivedaily/2008/10/casual_fridays_tk421_why_cant.php. Retrieved 2009-07-04.
- ^ Troje N F, McAdam M (2010-11-14). "The viewing-from-above bias and the silhouette illusion". i-Perception 1(3) 143-148. http://i-perception.perceptionweb.com/fulltext/i01/i0408.pdf. Retrieved 2010-11-14.
- ^ Kattinakere, Ragu (2008-02-03). "Spinning lady explained". http://thoughtslot.blogspot.com/2008/02/spinning-nude-lady.html. Retrieved 2008-02-03.
Categories:- Dance animation
- Optical illusions
- Perception
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.