Basil Bernstein

Basil Bernstein

Basil Bernstein (1 November1924-24 September2000) was a British sociologist and linguist, known for his work in the sociology of education.

Biography

He was born into a Jewish immigrant family, in the East End of London. He was late in coming to an academic career, gaining a doctorate aged nearly 40, after work including teaching and social work. He became Karl Mannheim Chair of the Sociology of Education, Institute of Education, University of London.

Theory of Language Code

Basil Bernstein made a significant contribution to the study of communication with his sociolinguistic theory of language codes. Within the broader category of language codes are elaborated and restricted codes. The term "code", as defined by Stephen Littlejohn in "Theories of Human Communication" (2002), “refers to a set of organizing principles behind the language employed by members of a social group” (p.278). Littlejohn (2002) suggests that Bernstein’s theory shows how the language people use in everyday conversation both reflects and shapes the assumptions of a certain social group. Furthermore, relationships established within the social group affect the way that group uses language, and the type of speech that is used.

According to James Atherton of the Doceo Teaching and Learning Website, the construct of restricted and elaborated language codes was introduced by Basil Bernstein in 1971. As an educator, he was interested in accounting for the relatively poor performance of working-class students in language-based subjects, when they were achieving scores as high as their middle-class counterparts on mathematical topics. In his theory, Bernstein makes a direct correlation between societal class and language.

According to Bernstein in "Class, Codes and Control (1971)",

:“Forms of spoken language in the process of their learning initiate, generalize and reinforce special types of relationship with the environment and thus create for the individual particular forms of significance” (p.76). That is to say that the way language is used within a particular societal class affects the way people assign significance and meaning to the things about which they are speaking. Littlejohn (2002) agrees and states, “people learn their place in the world by virtue of the language codes they employ” (p.178). The code that a person uses indeed symbolizes their social identity (Bernstein, 1971).

The two types of language codes are the elaborated code and the restricted code. Yhe restricted code does not refer to restricted vocabulary just as elaborated code does not refer to better, more eloquent language. According to Atherton (2002),

the essence of the distinction is in what the language is suited for. The restricted code works better than the elaborated code for situations in which there is a great deal of shared and taken-for-granted knowledge in the group of speakers. It is economical and rich, conveying a vast amount of meaning with a few words, each of which has a complex set of connotations and acts like an index, pointing the hearer to a lot more information which remains unsaid.

Within the restricted code, speakers draw on background knowledge and shared understanding. This type of code creates a sense of includedness, a feeling of belonging to a certain group. Restricted codes can be found among friends and families and other intimately knit groups.

Conversely, according to Atherton (2002), “the elaborated code spells everything out, not because it is better, but because it is necessary so that everyone can understand it. It has to elaborate because the circumstances do not allow the speaker to condense.” The elaborated code works well in situations where there is no prior or shared understanding and knowledge, where more thorough explanation is required. If one is saying something new to someone they’ve never met before, they would most certainly communicate in elaborated code.

In differentiating between restricted and elaborated codes, it is noted that elaborated code can “stand on its own”, it is complete and full of detail, most overhearing a conversation would be able to understand it. However, restricted code is shorter, condensed and requires background information and prior knowledge. A person overhearing a conversation full of restricted code would be quite lost. It would be easily identifiable as an “insiders” conversation. According to Bernstein (1971), “Clearly one code is not better than another; each possesses its own aesthetic, its own possibilities. Society, however, may place different values on the orders of experience elicited, maintained and progressively strengthened through the different coding systems” (p.135).

As communication occurs in groups and either the elaborated or restricted code is used, there is a degree of openness that is noticed. There is both the closed-role system and the open-role system. In a closed-role system, roles are set and people are viewed in terms of these roles, as well as expected to act in accordance with their role. In an open-role system, roles are not set or simple, they are fluid and changeable (Littlejohn, 2002).

There are two factors which contribute to the development of either an elaborated or restricted code within a system. They are: the nature of the socializing agencies (family, peer group, school, work) present in a system as well as the values within the system. When the socializing agencies are well defined and structured you find a restricted code. Conversely, where the agencies are malleable, an elaborated code is found. In a society which values individuality you find elaborated codes, and in a narrower society you find restricted codes (Littlejohn, 2002). Bernstein (1971) purports that, “The orientation towards these codes may be governed entirely by the form of the social relation, or more generally by the quality of the social structure” (p.135).

Bernstein makes a correlation between social class and the use of either elaborated or restricted code. He reports that in the working class you are likely to find the use of the restricted code, whereas in the middle class you find the use of both the restricted and elaborated codes. His research argues that the working class have access only to restricted codes, the ones they learned in the socialization process, where “both the values and role systems reinforce restricted codes” (Littlejohn, 2002 p.179). However, the middle class, being more geographically, socially and culturally mobile has access to both the restricted codes and elaborate codes. (Atherton, 2002). The restricted code is less formal with shorter phrases interjected into the middle or end of a thought to confirm understanding. For example, “you know,” “you know what I mean,“ “right?” and “don’t you think?” Elaborated codes have a longer, more complicated sentence structure that utilizes uncommon words and thoughts. In the elaborate code there is no padding or filler, only complete, well laid out thoughts that require no previous knowledge on the part of the listener, i.e., necessary details will be provided. According to Bernstein (1971), a working class person communicates in restricted code as a result of the conditions in which they were raised and the socialization process. The same is true for the middle class person with the exception that they were exposed to the elaborate code as well. Both groups use restricted code at some point, for as Atherton (2002) points out, “Everyone uses restricted code communication some of the time. It would be a very peculiar and cold family which did not have its own language.”

[The correlation between societal class and language codes shown herein explains for the poor performance in language based subjects by the working class students mentioned earlier.]

Though Bernstein's sociolinguistic work on 'restricted code' and 'elaborated code' is widely known it represents only his very earliest work. This early work was the subject of considerable misunderstanding and controversy. Bernstein emphasised that 'code’ was not dialect and that code theory was neither a bourgeois alibi for middle-class speech nor a denigrating deficit account of working-class language.

Bernstein's 'code theory' in the sociology of education has undergone considerable development since the early 1970s and now enjoys a growing influence in both education and linguistics, especially among systemic functional linguists. Maton & Muller (2007) describe how Bernstein argued that different positions within society, understood in terms of their degree of specialization, create, as he later put it, ‘different modalities of communication differentially valued by the school, and differentially effective in it, because of the school’s values, modes of practice and relations with its different communities’ (1996: 91). The notion was codified first in terms of "classification" and "framing", where classification conceptualises relations of power that regulate relations between contexts or categories, and framing conceptualises relations of control within these contexts or categories (1975). These concepts have been widely used to analyse educational contexts and practices and their relations to the dispositions (or coding orientation) brought to education by different social groups.

These concepts raised the question of how different forms of educational knowledge are constructed. Bernstein’s answer was: by means of the pedagogic device (see Maton & Muller 2007). This forms the basis of his account of: the ordered regulation and distribution of a society’s worthwhile knowledge store (ordered by a set of distributive rules); its transformation into a pedagogic discourse, a form amenable to pedagogic transmission (ordered by a specifiable set of recontextualising rules); and the further transformation of this pedagogic discourse into a set of criterial standards to be attained (ordered by a specifiable set of evaluative rules). In Bernstein’s conceptualisation each of these rules is associated with a specific field of activity: a field of production where ‘new’ knowledge is constructed and positioned; a field of recontextualisation where discourses from the field of production are selected, appropriated and repositioned to become ‘educational’ knowledge; and a field of reproduction where pedagogic practice takes place. Together these three rules and their associated fields constitute an ‘arena’ of conflict and struggle created by the pedagogic device in which social groups attempt to dominate how educational knowledge is constructed:

Groups attempt to appropriate the device to impose their rule by the construction of particular code modalities. Thus the device or apparatus becomes the focus of challenge, resistance and conflict(Bernstein 1996: 193).

As Moore & Maton (2001) describe, having analysed the nature of educational knowledge, and then how knowledge is selected from fields of knowledge production and then rearranged and recontextualised to become educational knowledge, the next question is: what characterises the nature of these fields of knowledge production? Bernstein conceptualises these in terms of 'knowledge structures'. Bernstein defines a "hierarchical knowledge structure" as ‘a coherent, explicit and systematically principled structure, hierarchically organised’ which ‘attempts to create very general propositions and theories, which integrate knowledge at lower levels, and in this way shows underlying uniformities across an expanding range of apparently different phenomena’ (1999: 161, 162), such as physics. A "horizontal knowledge structure" is defined as ‘a series of specialised languages with specialised modes of interrogation and criteria for the construction and circulation of texts’ (1999: 162), such as each of the disciplines of the humanities and social sciences.

These concepts have formed the basis for a growing range of studies into knowledge, both inside and outside education and across both knowledge production and teaching and learning (see, for example, Christie & Martin eds 2007; Maton 2000).

Works

* "Theoretical Studies Towards A Sociology Of Language" (1971)
* "Applied Studies Towards A Sociology Of Language" (1973)
* "Selection And Control" (1974) with Walter Landis
* "Towards A Theory Of Educational Transmissions" (1975)
* "The Structuring Of Pedagogic Discourse" (1990)
* "Social Class, Language And Communication" with Dorothy Henderson
* "Pedagogy, Symbolic Control and Identity" (1996 / 2000 second edition)

Primary Sources

*The personal papers of Basil Bernstein are held in Archives of the Institute of Education, University of London [http://ioewebserver.ioe.ac.uk/ioe/cms/get.asp?cid=9347&9347_0=14832] .

References

* Atkinson, P. (1985) "Language, Structure and Reproduction: An introduction to the sociology of Basil Bernstein." London, Methuen.
* Atkinson, P., Delamont, S. & Davies, B. (1995) (eds) "Discourse and Reproduction: Essays in honor of Basil Bernstein". Cresskill, NJ, Hampton Press.
* Christie, F. (1999) (ed) "Pedagogy and the Shaping of Consciousness: Linguistic and social processes". London, Continuum.
* Christie, F. & Martin, J. (2007) (eds) "Language, Knowledge and Pedagogy: Functional linguistic and sociological perspectives". London, Continuum.
* Maton, K. (2000) Recovering pedagogic discourse: A Bernsteinian approach to the sociology of educational knowledge, "Linguistics & Education" 11 (1), 79-98. [http://www.karlmaton.com]
* Maton, K. & Muller, J. (2007) A sociology for the transmission of knowledges, in Christie, F. & Martin, J. (eds) "Language, Knowledge and Pedagogy". London, Continuum, 14-33.
* Moore, R., Arnot, M., Beck, J. & Daniels, H. (eds) (2006) "Knowledge, Power and Educational Reform: Applying the sociology of Basil Bernstein". London, Routledge.
* Moore, R. & Maton, K. (2001) Founding the sociology of knowledge: Basil Bernstein, intellectual fields and the epistemic device, in Morais, A., Neves, I., Davies, B. & Daniels, H. (Eds.) "Towards a Sociology of Pedagogy". New York, Peter Lang, 153-182.
* Morais, A., Neves, I., Davies, B. & Daniels, H. (2001) (eds) "Towards a Sociology of Pedagogy: The contribution of Basil Bernstein to research". New York, Peter Lang.
* Muller, J., Davies, B. & Morais, A. (2004) (eds) "Reading Bernstein, Researching Bernstein". London, RoutledgeFalmer.
* Sadovnik, A.R. (ed) (1995) "Knowledge and Pedagogy: The sociology of Basil Bernstein". Norwood, NJ, Ablex.

External links

* [http://www.guardian.co.uk/obituaries/story/0,3604,373772,00.html Obituary in "The Guardian"]
* [http://www.ibe.unesco.org/publications/ThinkersPdf/bernsteine.pdf Obituary by Alan R. Sadovnik]
* [http://ioewebserver.ioe.ac.uk/ioe/cms/get.asp?cid=1397&1397_1=1698 Institute obituary notice]
* [http://zimmer.csufresno.edu/~johnca/spch100/3-3-bernstein.htm]
* [http://www.ioe.ac.uk/library/archives/bbb.html Basil Bernstein, Bibliography]
* [http://tech.groups.yahoo.com/group/B_Bernstein/ Discussion group on Bernstein's sociological approach]


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Basil Bernstein — Saltar a navegación, búsqueda Basil Bernstein Nacimiento 1 de noviembre de 1924 …   Wikipedia Español

  • Basil Bernstein — Basil Bernard Bernstein (* 1. November 1924; † 24. September 2000) war ein britischer Soziolinguist. Basil Bernstein stellte in den 1960er Jahren die Defizithypothese in der Soziolinguistik auf. Er stellte die Theorie auf, dass die Sprache der… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Basil Bernstein — Pour les articles homonymes, voir Bernstein. Basil Bernstein Pays de résidence   …   Wikipédia en Français

  • Clases, códigos y control I. Estudios teóricos para una sociología del lenguaje. Basil Bernstein — Autor Basil Bernstein Resumen Bernstein, en su libro, se dedica a establecer diferencias entre las clases sociales, atribuidas al lenguaje. El libro está organizado en tres partes que contienen una recopilación de artículos escritos por el mismo… …   Wikipedia Español

  • Basil Bernard Bernstein — (* 1. November 1924; † 24. September 2000) war ein britischer Soziolinguist. Basil Bernstein stellte in den 1960 er Jahren die Defizithypothese in der Soziolinguistik auf. Er stellte die Theorie auf, dass die Sprache der Unterschicht defizitär… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Bernstein (Familienname) — Bernstein ist ein Familienname. Bekannte Personen Inhaltsverzeichnis A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Basil — ist ein männlicher Vorname[1] und ein Familienname. Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Herkunft und Bedeutung 2 Bekannte Namensträger 2.1 Vorname 2.2 Familien …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Bernstein (Begriffsklärung) — Bernstein steht für: Bernstein, ein fossiles Harz Bernstein (Familienname), mehrere Personen mit dem Familienname Bernstein Bernstein (Band), eine Musikgruppe aus Limburg Betty Bernstein, Comicfigur und Maskottchen Bernstein AG,… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Bernstein — Saltar a navegación, búsqueda El apellido Bernstein corresponde a varias personalidades: Basil Bernstein, sociólogo y lingüista británico Carl Bernstein, periodista estadounidense que, con Bob Woodward, descubrió el escándalo del Watergate;… …   Wikipedia Español

  • Bernstein-Hypothese — Die Bernstein Hypothese, auch Defizithypothese, ist eine 1958 von Basil Bernstein entwickelte linguistische Annahme. Bernstein baut dabei auf der Sapir Whorf Hypothese auf, verschiebt deren Fokus aber auf innersprachliche Betrachtungen, also auf… …   Deutsch Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”