- Psychohistorical views on infanticide
:
"This article is an expansion of a section entitled "Psychological" from within the main article:
Infanticide "Psychohistorical views on infanticide (in psychohistorical jargon "early infanticidal childrearing"") is a model used in the study of
psychohistory to refer to the occurrence ofinfanticide inpaleolithic , [Decapitated skeletons ofhominid children have been found with evidence of cannibalism. See e.g., cite journal | last = Simons | first = E.L. | title = Human origins | journal =Science (journal) | volume = 245 | pages = 1344 | year = 1989] [cite book | last = Maringer | first = Johannes | title = The Gods of Prehistoric Man | publisher = Weidenfield & Nicolson | year = 1956 | location = London | pages = 10-19] pre-historical, or historical hunter-gatherertribe s or societies (although infanticide still occurs in the advanced nations). "Early" means early in history or in the cultural development of a society, not to the age of the child. "Infanticidal" refers to the high incidence of infants killed if compared to modern nations.cite book | last = deMause | first = Lloyd | title = The Emotional Life of Nations
publisher = Karnak | year = 2002
location = NY/London] The model was developed byLloyd deMause within the framework of psychohistory as part of a seven-stage sequence of childrearing modes which describe the development of human cultures in their attitude to their children. ["The Emotional Life of Nations" (op. cit.), [http://www.geocities.com/kidhistory/childhod/chch72dm.htm Chapter 7, Part 2, "Childhood and Cultural Evolution"] .] The word "early" is also meant to distinguish it fromlate infanticidal childrearing , identified by deMause in the more established, agricultural cultures up to theancient world .The model
This particular model is a psychological concept that aims to understand anthropological data, especially from such societies as the
Yolngu ofAustralia , theGimi ,Wogeo ,Bena Bena , andBimin-Kuskusmin ofPapua New Guinea , the Raum, the Ok and the Kwanga, based on observations byGeza Roheim , [cite book
last = Roheim
first = Geza
title = Psychoanalysts and Anthropology
publisher = International Universities Press
year = 1950
location = NY] Lia Leibowitz,Robert C. Suggs , [cite book
last = Suggs
first = Robert C.
title = Marquesan Sexual Behavior
publisher = Hartcourt, Brace & World
year = 1966
location = NY]Milton Diamond , Herman Heinrich Ploss,Gilbert Herdt , Robert J. Stoller, L. L. Langness, and Fitz John Porter Poole, among others.cite journal
last = deMause
first = Lloyd
title = On Writing Childhood History
journal = The Journal of Psychohistory
volume = 16 (2) Fall
year = 1988 [http://www.psychohistory.com/childhood/writech1.htm] ] While anthropologists and psychohistorians generally do not dispute the data of their particular research, they dispute its significance (both in terms of importance and in terms of meaning) and its interpretation.Supporters attempt to explain
cultural history from a psycho-developmental point of view, and argue that cultural change can be assessed as "advancement" or "regression" based on the psychological consequences of various cultural practices. [ cite journal
last = deMause
first = Lloyd
title = The evolution of childrearing modes
journal = Empathic Parenting
volume = 15, issues 1 & 2
year= 1992 ] While most anthropologists reject this approach, and most theories ofcultural evolution asethnocentric , the psychohistorians in their turn proclaim the independence of psychohistory and summarily reject the mainstream view in scholarship, that of Boasian anthropologists.This "infanticidal" model makes several claims: that childrearing in tribal societies included
child sacrifice or high infanticide rates,incest , body mutilation, child rape and tortures, and that such activities were culturally acceptable. [cite book | last = Rascovsky | first = A. | title = Filicide: The Murder, Humiliation, Mutilation, Denigration and Abandonment of Children by Parents | publisher = Aronson | year = 1995 | location = NJ | pages = 107] Psychohistorians do not claim that each child was killed, only that in some societies there was (or is) a selection process that would vary from culture to culture. For example, there is a large jump in the mortality rate of Papua New Guinean children after they reach the weaning stage.cite book
last = Milner
first = Larry S.
title = Hardness of Heart / Hardness of Life: The Stain of Human Infanticide
publisher = University Press of America
year = 2000 ] In theSolomon Islands some people reportedly kill their first-born child. In ruralIndia , ruralChina and other societies some female babies would be exposed to death. DeMause's argument is that the surviving siblings of the sacrificed child may become disturbed.Some states both in the
Old World andNew World also practiced infanticide, including sacrifice in Mesoamerica, the Incas and inAssyria n andCanaan ite religions. Phoenicians, Carthaginians and other members of early states also sacrificed infants to their gods, as described in the table of the psychopathological effects of some forms of childrearing.According to deMause, in the most primitive mode of childrearing of the above-mentioned table, mothers use their children to project parts of their dissociated self onto their children. The infanticidal clinging of the symbiotic mother prevents individuation so that innovation and more complex political organization are inhibited. On a second plane, supporters maintain that the attention paid by mothers of contemporary primitive tribes to their children, such as
sucking ,fondling andmasturbating issexual according to an objective standard; and that this sexual attention is inordinate. [ cite journal
last = deMause
first = Lloyd
title = The universality of incest
journal = The Journal of psychohistory
volume = 19, No. 2
year= 1991 ]The model is also based on a reported lack of empathy by infanticidal parents, such as mutual gazes between parent and child, observed by Robert B. Edgerton, Maria Lepowsky, Bruce Knauft, John W. M. Whiting and
Margaret Mead among others. Such mutual gazing is widely recognized indevelopmental psychology as crucial for proper bonding between mother and child.Criticism
Nineteenth century British anthropology advanced a lineal, evolutionary sequence in a given culture from savagery to civilization (Stone Age, Iron Age, Bronze Age, etc.). The cultures were seen as a hierarchical ladder. For example,
James George Frazer posited an universal progress frommagical thinking to science. Most anthropologists of the late nineteenth century and early twentieth century studied primitives outside Europe and North America.John Ferguson McLennan ,Lewis Henry Morgan and others argued that there was a parallel development in social institutions. Led byLeslie White , in the 1950s these evolutionist ideas gained influence in American anthropology.cite book
title = Anthropology
publisher =Encyclopædia Britannica 2007 Ultimate Reference Suite
location = Chicago]The German-born
Franz Boas managed to shift the paradigm. His approach, later namedcultural relativism , resists universal values of any kind. According to Boas' principle, which represents the mainstream school in contemporary anthropology, a set of a culture's beliefs and activities should be interpreted in terms of its ownculture . This principle has been established asaxiom atic in contemporary anthropology. The war of Vietnam consolidated the Boasian shift in American anthropology.Since the psychohistorians' model is analogous to the now discarded
unilineal evolution theory, anthropologists have been critical of the negativevalue judgment s, and the lineal progression, in the model currently advanced by psychohistorians as to what constituteschild abuse in either "primitive" or non-Western cultures. [cite journal
last = Paul
first = Robert A.
title = Review of Lloyd deMause's Foundations of Psychohistory
journal = Journal of Psychoanalytic Anthropology
volume = 5
pages = 469
year = 1982] Furthermore, lumping paleolithic societies with contemporary hunter-gatherers, as deMause and the psychohistorians do, is for anthropologists bad science. This view was rejected by anthropologists a hundred years ago.Melvin Konner wrote:Lloyd deMause, then editor of the "History of Childhood Quarterly", claimed that all past societies treated children brutally, and that all historical change in their treatment has been a fairly steady improvement toward the kind and gentle standards we now set and more or less meet. [...] Now anthropologists — and many historians as well — were slack-jawed and nearly speechless. [...] Serious students of the anthropology of childhood beginning with Margaret Mead have called attention to the pervasive love and care lavished on children in many traditional cultures. [cite book | last = Konner | first = Melvin | authorlink= Melvin Konner|title = Childhood | publisher = Little Brown & Co. | year = 1991 | location = Boston | pages = 193]
In return, psychohistorians accuse most anthropologists and ethnologists of having avoided looking more closely to the evidence and having promulgated the myth of the
noble savage .cite book | last = Godwin | first = Robert | title = One Cosmos under God | publisher = Omega Books | year= 2004 | pages = 166-174] They maintain that what constitutes child abuse is a matter of a general psychological law and that some of the practices which mainstream anthropologists do not pay due attention, such as beatings of newborn infants, result in brain lesions and other visible neurological and psychological damage.ee also
*
Child sacrifice in pre-Columbian cultures
*Cultural relativism
*Moral absolutism
*Religious abuse Notes
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.