- Rubin vase
Rubin's vase (sometimes known as the Rubin face or the Figure-ground vase) is a famous set of cognitive
optical illusion s developed around 1915 by the Danishpsychologist Edgar Rubin . They were first introduced at large in Rubin's two-volume work, the Danish-language "Synsoplevede Figurer" ("Visual Figures"), which was very well-received; Rubin included a number of examples, like aMaltese cross figure in black and white, but the one that became the most famous was his vase example, perhaps because the Maltese cross one could also be easily interpreted as a black and whitebeachball .The illusion
The illusion generally presents the viewer with a mental choice of two interpretations, each of which is valid. Often, the viewer sees only one of them, and only realizes the second, valid, interpretation after some time or prompting. When they attempt to simultaneously see the second and first interpretations, they suddenly cannot see the first interpretation anymore, and no matter how they try, they simply cannot encompass both interpretations simultaneously- one occludes the other.
Explanation
The illusions are useful because they are an excellent and intuitive demonstration of the
figure-ground distinction the brain makes duringvisual perception . Rubin's figure-ground distinction, since it involved higher-level cognitivepattern matching , in which the overall picture determines its mental interpretation, rather than the net effect of the individual pieces, influenced theGestalt psychologists, who discovered many similar illusions themselves.Normally the brain classifies images by what surrounds what- establishing depth and relationships. If something surrounds another thing, the surrounded object is seen as figure, and the presumably further away (and hence background) object is the ground, and vice versa. This makes sense, since if a piece of fruit is lying on the ground, one would want to pay attention to the "figure" and not the "ground". However, when the contours are not so unequal, ambiguity starts to creep into the previously simple inequality, and the brain must begin "shaping" what it sees; it can be shown that this shaping overrides and is at a higher level than feature recognition processes that pull together the face and the vase images- one can think of the lower levels putting together distinct regions of the picture (each region of which makes sense in isolation), but when the brain takes to make sense of it as a whole, contradictions ensue, and patterns must be discarded.
Construction
The distinction is exploited by devising an ambiguous picture, whose contours match seamlessly the contours of another picture (sometimes the same picture; a practice
M. C. Escher used on occasion) or more often another picture. The picture should be "flat" and have little (if any) texture to it. The stereotypical example has a vase in the center, and a face matching its contour (since it is symmetrical, there is a matching face on the other side).References
* "A Psychology of Picture Perception", John M. Kennedy. 1974, Jossey-Bass Publishers, ISBN 0-87589-204-3
* "The art and science of visual illusions", Nicholas Wade. 1982 Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. ISBN 0-7100-0868-6
* "Visual Space Perception", William H. Ittelson. 1969, Springer Publishing Company, LOCCCN 60-15818
* "Vase or face? A neural correlates of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain." Uri Hasson, Talma Hendler, Dafna Ben Bashat, Rafael Malach.
*"Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience", Vol 13(6), Aug 2001. pp. 744-753. ISSN 0898-929X (Print)External links
* [http://www.moillusions.com/2006/04/people-trapped-inside-wall.html Rubin's People Inside the Wall] People trapped inside a Wall
* [http://psylux.psych.tu-dresden.de/i1/kaw/diverses%20Material/www.illusionworks.com/html/figure_ground.html Illusionworks.com article]
* [http://figuresambigues.free.fr/ArticlesImage/rubin1.html Rubin has invented nothing] The Rubin's vase before Rubin (fr)
Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.