Life history theory

Life history theory

Life history theory is an analytical framework widely used in animal and human biology, psychology, and evolutionary anthropology which postulates that many of the physiological traits and behaviors of individuals may be best understood in terms of the key maturational and reproductive characteristics that define the life course.

Examples of these characteristics include:
*Age at weaning
*Age of sexual maturity or puberty
*Adult body size
*Age specific mortality schedules
*Age specific fecundity
*Time to first sexual activity or mating
*Time to first reproduction
*Duration of gestation
*Litter size
*Interbirth interval

Variations in these characteristics reflect differing allocations of an individual's resources (i.e., time, effort, and energy expenditure) to competing life functions, especially growth, body maintenance, and reproduction. For any given individual, available resources in any particular environment are finite. Time, effort, and energy used for one purpose diminishes the time effort, and energy available for another. For example, resources spent growing to a larger body size cannot be spent increasing the number of offspring. In general terms the costs of reproduction may be paid in terms of energy being diverted away from body repair and maintenance and by reducing investment in immunological competence.

Thus the allocation of resources involves trade-offs. These trade-offs and strategies can be compared between species. Two of the most well-known trade-offs involve number of offspring (few or many) and timing of reproduction (accelerated maturation and reproduction versus delayed, allowing for larger size and more complex social supports). Anthony Zera and Zhangwn Zhao have recognized a prime example of trade-offs amongst female sand crickets. Female sand crickets are easily recognized as being long or short winged. Adult long winged females store excess triglycerides as an energy source to fuel well developed flight muscles. This trait is adaptive, providing the insect with the ability to find more suitable environments when necessary. The downfall of this is represented by the decreased rate of growth of ovaries. In the short winged females, triglyceride storage is limited and flight muscles are underdeveloped. Energy saved will be used to produce phospholipids, utilized in egg production. The following represents a trade-off experienced amongst female sand crickets, involving “dispersal ability versus early reproduction.” (Freeman 485-86) The extremes at the species level of these fundamental dimensions of reproduction were recognized long before life history theory, and are traditionally termed r/K selection theory. An r-selection strategy is the production of a large number of offspring (of whom only a minority may survive) as early in life as possible. The K-selection strategy is to produce a smaller number of "fitter" offspring with higher survival chances.

According to life history theory the individuals of a species are able to make limited shifts in reproductive strategies in response to the prevailing environments. Depending on abundance of resources and probable individual longevity, individuals consciously or unconsciously shift their reproductive strategy in one direction or the other to take advantage of available resources or to compensate for resource shortage or uncertainty.

History of Life history theory

Traditional Studies

Initially life history theory postulated that life history characteristics were essentially 'extrinsic' (Charnov, 1993, Kozlowski & Wiegert, 1986) and not subject to endogenous processes. However this approach has more recently been found to be theoretically unsatisfying, as there is evidence that higher organisms exert control over virtually all causes of mortality (e.g., by altering patterns of travel to avoid predators, by investing in immune function etc) and these endogenous processes can also be seen at work in the variance in ages of menarché and menopause (Kaplan and Robson, 2002, Kaplan et al, 2003). This argument has some resonance with more recent economic approaches, notably Robert Fogel and Dora Costa’s idea of technophysio evolution (Fogel, 2004). The traditional life history framework has also been thought to be analytically limited, in that it does not allow for a full understanding of how mortality rates evolve. In reality what varies as a function of ecological factors are not set mortality rates, but rather functional relationships between mortality and efforts allocated to reducing it. Exogenous variation can be thought of in terms of varying 'assault' types and rates (Kaplan et al, 2003).

Recent Studies

More recently researchers working in the life history tradition have drawn attention to the way the functional relationships described may also be understood in terms of the fetal programming hypothesis. This hypothesis holds that early growth retardation results in adjustments or impairments in fetal development which have permanent consequences for function and health risk. Programming is thought to occur through "induction, deletion or impaired development of a permanent somatic structure as a result of a stimulus or insult" (Davies and Norman, 2002:386). An accompanying mechanism has also been advanced in an attempt to explain the operation of the programming: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. The thrifty phenotype process is thought to operate via altered glucose-insulin metabolism (Hales and Barker, 1992, 2001). Although evidence of fetal programming in humans has been questioned on the basis of weaknesses in retrospective epidemiological studies (Huxley et al, 2002), there is very strong evidence from studies of animals to show that an adverse environment (including maternal nutrition) in fetal or neonatal life can have profound effects on metabolic and cardiovascular disease risk in adulthood (Armitage "et al", 2004, Langley Evans, 2006, McMillen and Robinson, 2005)

The fetal origins hypothesis has subsequently been developed to include the idea of an 'appropriateness of fit' between the phenotype and its environment. This has resulted in increased attention being drawn to the problems which may occur if there is a mismatch between physiologic capacities established in early development and the environments in which they later function.

The life history theory is also being examined in recent studies involving the human immune system. Concepts from life-history theory are applied to highlight the major challenges and demands that are likely to shape immune function in a range of ecological contexts. Immune function is a major component of maintenance effort, and since resources are limited, trade-offs are expected between investment in maintenance and other critical life-history functions involving growth and reproduction. An adaptationist, life-history perspective helps make sense of the unusual developmental trajectory of immune tissues, and emphasizes that this complex system is designed to incorporate information from the surrounding ecology to guide its development. As a result, there is substantial population variation in immune development and function that is not considered by current biomedical approaches. In an attempt to construct a framework for understanding this variation, immune development is considered in relation to the competing life-history demands that define gestation, infancy, childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Each life stage poses a unique set of adaptive challenges, and a series of hypotheses is proposed regarding their implications for immune development and function (McDade).

Life history theory

In life history terms, fetal programming works to 'configure' our somatic system so that in conditions of good maternal health, low juvenile morbidity rates, and sustained normal nutrition the 'settings' work just so as to increase overall productivity and reduce the relative cost of maintenance (the meaning of 'good health', 'low juvenile morbity' and 'normal nutrition' should be understood in terms of the pre-historic foraging environment where our biology effectively evolved). From this initial 'setting' any sustained increase in nutritional energy input (or reduction in the disease load on the immune system) tends towards increased growth and accelerated maturation (this incidentally may help understand those puzzling long-term fluctuations in population sizes and final heights which characterized the pre-industrial period). Such improved environmental circumstances are also know to operate indirectly on age at maturity since they act as a signal that resource availability is reliably good and that mortality risk is low. Hence it has been found that the norm of reaction for timing of maturation in humans is large, and documented population median ages at menarche range from 12.3–18 years (Eveleth and Tanner, 1990), and the mean age at menarche has been falling steadily in modern growth environments (Weil, 2005).

Thus the trade-offs identified by life history can be interpreted as implying that the present health consequences of differential fetal outcomes represent the resurfacing of costs deferred in early trade-offs. According to the fetal programming hypothesis permanent alterations in metabolic regulation under early conditions of adversity could be seen as adaptive in virtue the overarching need to produce an energy-sparing phenotype (Hales and Barker, 2001). The resulting conceptual problem has been to clarify why such permanent changes should be adaptive if they yield little in the way of future benefits, yet do exact a future cost. Perhaps the advantage is to trade off health in later life in order to allow an individual to grow to sexual maturity, mate successfully and perpetuate the bloodline. Once this is achieved, health in later life has minimal importance from an evolutionary perspective.

Perspectives

Life history theory has provided new perspectives in understanding many aspects of human reproductive behavior, such as the relationship between poverty and fertility. A number of statistical predictions have been confirmed by social data, though not always reproducibly. The implications for social policy have been hotly debated because statistical associations are not always causal, and a preferred interpretation may be a more minor factor than another unpalatable relationship. Nonetheless, there is a large body of scientific literature from studies in experimental animal models. In the main these studies support the notion that perturbations in the environment encountered during development may program changes in the fetus or neonate that later render it susceptible to disease.

Analyzing trade-offs among life history traits has helped to explain many aspects of the extraordinary life history diversity known among living organisms. However, this view can sometimes obscure the fact that the life history of each organism unfolds in an ecological context that includes other individuals. In fact, the reproductive interests of males and females will often be different. Analyzing trade-offs among life history traits has helped to explain many aspects of the extraordinary life history diversity known among living organisms. However, this view can sometimes obscure the fact that the life history of each organism unfolds in an ecological context that includes other individuals. In fact, the reproductive interests of males and females will often be different. This presents a conflict of interest between males and females. Experiments performed by William Rice and colleagues present when the reproductive interests of female and male fruit flies are different, sexual selection has the tendency to favor adaptations that come about in one sex but are not beneficial to the other sex. One example of these adaptations is the biological makeup of male seminal fluid. This has evolved the ability to influence the behavior of females, like their rate of egg-laying or potential to remate with other males. The effects of these adaptations favor the male if his female mate is not monogamous, because this will increase the amount of fertilized eggs with his sperm. However, this could lead to effects that are not beneficial to females, since male seminal fluid displays toxic effects and increases the mortality of females. This will tend to favor the evolution of resistance in females, while males will develop adaptations to conquer the resistance of females. This process is called chase-away sexual selection (Freeman 516).

ee also

* Behavioral ecology
* Evolutionary developmental psychology
* Evolutionary physiology
* Human behavioral ecology
* Parental investment
* Reproductive effort
**Mating effort
**Parental effort
**Nepotistic effort
* Somatic effort

References

*Armitage, J. A., Khan, I. Y., Taylor, P. D, Nathanielsz, P. W. and Poston, L. (2004). Developmental programming of the metabolic syndrome by maternal nutritional imbalance: how strong is the evidence from experimental models in animals. "Journal of Physiology" 561: 355-377.
*Charnov, E. L. (1993). "Life history invariants". Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
*Davies M.J., Norman R.J. 2002. "Programming and reproductive functioning". "Trends Endocrinol Metab" 13: 386–392.
*Eveleth P., and J Tanner, 1990. "Worldwide variation in human growth". New York: Cambridge University Press.
*Fogel, Robert W, 2001. Biotechnology and the burden of age-related diseases, Chicago Centre For Population Econonics, Working Paper WP- 200 -2
*Freeman, Scott and Herron, Jon C. 2007. Evolutionary Analysis 4th Ed: Aging and Other Life History Characteristics. 485-86, 514, 516.
*Hales CN, Barker DJP. 2001. "The thrifty phenotype hypothesis". "Br Med Bull" 60:5–20
*Hales CN, Barker DJP. 1992. Type 2 (non-insulindependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. "Diabetologia" 35:595–601.
*Huxley R., Neil A., Colins R. 2002. Unravelling the fetal origins hypothesis: is there really an inverse association between birthweight and subsequent blood pressure? Lancet 360: 659-665.
*Kozlowski, J and Wiegert, RG 1986. Optimal allocation to growth and reproduction. Theoretical Population, 29, 16-37.
*Kaplan, H.S., and AJ Robson, 2002. The emergence of humans: The coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers. "PNAS" 99(15):10221-10226.
* Kaplan, H.S., Lancaster, J.B., & Robson, 2003. Embodied Capital and the Evolutionary Economics Of the Human Lifespan. In: Lifespan: Evolutionary, Ecology and Demographic Perspectives, J.R. Carey & S. Tuljapakur (eds.) "Population and Development Review" 29, Supplement 2003, Pp. 152-182.
* Langley-Evans, S.C. (2006). Developmental programming of health and disease. "Proceedings of the Nutrition Society" 65: 97-105.
*Mc Millen I. C. and Robinson J. S. (2005). Developmental Origins of the Metabolic Syndrome: Prediction, Plasticity and Programming. "Physiological Reviews" 85; 571- 633.
*Roff, D. (1992). "The evolution of life histories: Theory and analysis". New York:Chapman & Hall.
* Selander, Robert K. (Sep., 1967). Why There are no Subspecies of North American House Sparrows "Systematic Zoology" Vol. 16, No. 3, pp. 286-287
*Stearns, S. (1992). "The evolution of life histories". Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
*Weil, David N, 2005, Accounting for the Effect of Health on Economic Growth, NBER Working Paper No. 11455McDade, TW. American Journal of Phys. Anthropology; 2003.

Further reading

*Ellis, B.J. (2004). [http://ag.arizona.edu/fcs/fshd/people/ellis/Psyc%20Bull%20Ellis%202004.pdf Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: an integrated life history approach.] "Psychological Bulletin." 130:920-58.

*Kaplan, H., K. Hill, J. Lancaster, and A.M. Hurtado. (2000). [http://www.unm.edu/~kimhill/papers/kaplan2000a.pdf The Evolution of intelligence and the Human life history] . Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(4): 156-184..

* Langley-Evans, S.C. (2004). Fetal nutrition and adult disease; programming of chronic disease through fetal exposure to undernutrition. CABI Publishing, Wallingford, UK.

*Quinlan, R.J. (2007). [http://www.wsu.edu/~rquinlan/Quinlan_PRSB.pdf Human parental effort and environmental risk.] Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1606):121-125.

* Vigil, J. M., Geary, D. C., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2005). [http://web.missouri.edu/~psycorie/Vigiletal%5BDP2005%5D.pdf A life history assessment of early childhood sexual abuse in women.] Developmental Psychology, 41, 553-561.

*Walker, R., Gurven, M., Hill, K., Migliano, A., Chagnon, N., Djurovic, G., Hames, R., Hurtado, AM, Kaplan, H., Oliver, W., de Souza, R., Valeggia, C., Yamauchi, T. (2006). [http://www.anth.ucsb.edu/faculty/gurven/ Growth rates, developmental markers and life histories in 21 small-scale societies] . American Journal of Human Biology 18:295-311.

*Derek A. Roff (2007). [http://www.nature.com/nrg/journal/v8/n2/abs/nrg2040.html Contributions of genomics to life-history theory] . Nature Reviews Genetics 8, 116-125.


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Life-history-Theorie — Die funktionelle Differenzierung des Lebensaufwands.[1] Die Life history Theorie (engl. life history theory), seltener auch Theorie der Lebensgeschichte genannt[2], postuliert, dass sich Organismen begrenzte …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • Life history — The term life history has been given many meanings in several scientific fields. It can refer to a variety of methods and techniques that are used for conducting qualitative interviews, especially in the fields of sociology and… …   Wikipedia

  • life-history — An ideographic approach which provides an intensive account of a life, usually gathered through unstructured interviewing , but often also involving the analysis of personal documents such as letters, photographs, and diaries. The approach is… …   Dictionary of sociology

  • Life Orientations Training — Life Orientations Training, or LIFO Training for short, is an applied behavioral science system designed to foster individual and organizational productivity. Common applications include leadership development, interpersonal communication skills …   Wikipedia

  • History of evolutionary thought — This article is about the history of evolutionary thought in biology. For the history of evolutionary thought in the social sciences, see Sociocultural evolution. For the history of religious discussions, see History of the creation evolution… …   Wikipedia

  • Life Sciences — ▪ 2009 Introduction Zoology       In 2008 several zoological studies provided new insights into how species life history traits (such as the timing of reproduction or the length of life of adult individuals) are derived in part as responses to… …   Universalium

  • Life expectancy — This article is about the measure of remaining life. For the Dean Koontz novel, see Life Expectancy (novel). For List of countries by life expectancy, see List of countries by life expectancy. Life expectancy is the expected (in the statistical… …   Wikipedia

  • History of psychology — The history of psychology as a scholarly study of the mind and behavior dates back to the Ancient Greeks. There is also evidence of psychological thought in ancient Egypt. Psychology was a branch of philosophy until the 1870s, when psychology… …   Wikipedia

  • History of childhood — The history of childhood has been a topic of interest in social history since the 1960s. Playing Children, by Song Dynasty Chinese artist Su Hanchen, c. 1150 AD. Contents 1 …   Wikipedia

  • LIFE SCIENCES — Introduction Biology has become a vast subject which has increasingly merged with traditionally separate disciplines, particularly chemistry and physics. Indeed life sciences is now a more appropriate term than biology. Furthermore the life… …   Encyclopedia of Judaism

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”