Covariation model

Covariation model

Contents

Introduction

Kelley’s covariation model (1967, 1971, 1972, 1973)[1] is an attribution theory in which people make causal inferences to explain why other people and ourselves behave in a certain way. It is concerned with both social perception and self-perception (Kelley, 1973).

The covariation principle states that, "an effect is attributed to the one of its possible causes with which, over time, it covaries" (Kelley, 1973:108). That is, a certain behaviour is attributed to potential causes that appear at the same time. This principle is useful when the individual has the opportunity to observe the behaviour over several occasions[2]. The number of observations made can be as small as two[3] or more than two[4]. The three causes which behaviour is attributed to are Person (actor), Object (action) and Context (situation), and attribution is made based on three criteria: Consensus, Distinctiveness and Consistency (Kelley, 1973).

Distinctiveness

Psychodynamics is another type of covariation information that is important in the effort of understanding the causes of social behaviour. Distinctiveness refers to how unique the psychodynamics is to the particular situation. There is a low distinctiveness if an individual behaves similarly in all situations, and there exists a high distinctiveness when the person only shows the behaviour in particular situations. If the distinctiveness is high, one will attribute this behaviour more to the context instead of person characteristics (Gilovich et al., 2005).

Referring to the example of Dr. Stanton’s complimenting Barry’s work, if Dr. Stanton almost never compliments other people’s work, he shows high distinctiveness. But if he also compliments everybody’s work, this is low distinctiveness, and one will attribute the behaviour to some personal characteristics in Dr. Stanton (Orvis et al., 1975).

Consistency

Consistency is the continuous behaviour of something. For example, if Mrs. Browning's 3rd period class maintains a continuous behavior of growing the friendship of Kate and Jane, this is consistency.

Making attribution with the three sources of information

By putting the three sources of information, consensus, distinctiveness and consistency, together, we are able to determine whether a person would likely to make an attribution to object, person or context (Orvis et al., 1975).

Let us consider the following example. When a girl went into a boutique and tried on a dress, the salesman told the girl that the dress looked very nice on her. The girl might have the following three different types of attribution towards the salesman’s behavior.

If all the salesmen in the boutique told the girl that the dress was looked nice on her (consensus is high); if the salesman only said she looked nice in one particular dress (distinctiveness is high); if the salesman said so every time the girl went into the boutique (consistency is high), the girl will likely attribute the salesman’s behavior to the object. Thus, she would conclude that the salesman said so because the dress was nice.

If only one particular salesman told the girl that the dress was nice on her (consensus is low); if the salesman said she looked nice in other dresses in the boutique as well (distinctiveness is low); and if the salesman said so every time the girl went into the boutique (consistency is high), the girl was likely to attribute the salesman’s behavior to the person. Thus, she would conclude that the salesman said so because the salesman was nice.

If only one particular salesman told the girl that the dress looked nice on her (consensus is low); if the salesman only said she looked nice in one particular dress (distinctiveness is high); and if the salesman said so only this time the girl went into the boutique (consistency is low), the girl was likely to attribute the salesman’s behavior to the context. Thus, she would conclude that the salesman said so because he or she had to fulfill a sales quota on that dress.

Summary Table Information Patterns For The Three Attributions
Attribution Information pattern
Consensus Distinctiveness Consistency
Object High High High
Person Low Low High
Context Low High Low

Limitations

Kelley’s covariation model also has its limitations. The critique of the model mainly concerns the lack of distinction between intentional and unintentional behavior, and between reason and cause explanations (Malle, 1999).

Intentional behavior occurs when there is a desire for an outcome, together with a belief that a certain behavior will lead to the desired outcome. These beliefs and desires are mental states acting as reasons behind an intention to act. When behavior is unintentional, the behavior is not explained by reasons, but rather by cause explanations not related to mental states of desire and belief. Malle (1999) found that whether behavior is intentional or unintentional predicts the type of explanation, and that the type of explanation presented predicts the judgement of intentionality.

Malle (1999) also pointed at the differential effect of being an actor versus observer, the effect of the self-serving bias and the distinction between subjective and rational reasoning as important factors acting on attributions of behavior. This is not accounted for by the covariation model. Malle offers a new theoretical framework to give a broader and more comprehensive understanding of attributions of behavior.

Notes

  1. ^ also known as ‘ANOVA model’ (Kelley & Michela, 1980:462)
  2. ^ see also Kelley’s discounting principle (1971, 1972): single observation
  3. ^ as in the experiment in Kelley and Stahelski (1970), as quoted by Kelley (1973)
  4. ^ as in the experiment in Valins (1966), as quoted by Kelley (1973)

References

  • Gilovich T., Kelner D., Nisbett R. E. (2005). Social psychology. New York: Norton & Company.
  • Himmelfarb, S. (1972). Integration and attribution theories in personality impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23:309-13.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. New York: General Learning Press.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York: General Learning Press.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American psychologist, 28(2), 107-128.
  • Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research. Annual review of psychology, 31, 457-501.
  • Kelley, H. H., & Stahelski, A. J. (1970). The inference of intention from moves in the Prisoner’s Dilemma Game. Journal of experimental social psychology, 6, 401-419.
  • Malle, B. F. (1999). How people explain behavior: A new theoretical framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 1: 23-48
  • McArthur, L.A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22: 171-193.
  • Orvis, B.R., Cunningham, J.D. & Kelley, H.H. (1975). A closer examination of causal inference: The roles of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (4): 605-616.
  • Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. Journal of personality and social psychology, 4, 400-408.

Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем сделать НИР

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Attribution (psychology) — Attribution is a concept in social psychology referring to how individuals explain behaviors of others. Types of attribution The two main types of attributions are internal and external attributions. When an internal attribution is made, the… …   Wikipedia

  • Duck HBV RNA encapsidation signal epsilon — The Duck HBV RNA encapsidation signal epsilon is an RNA structure that is shown to facilitate encapsidation of the pregenomic RNA required for replication.DHBV is used as a model to understand human HBV. DHBV epsilon consists of a stem structure …   Wikipedia

  • Avian HBV RNA encapsidation signal epsilon — Fig. 1. Duck HBV RNA encapsidation signal epsilon Fig. 2. Heron HBV RNA encapsidation signal epsi …   Wikipedia

  • Attribution theory — is a social psychology theory developed by Fritz Heider, Harold Kelley, Edward E. Jones, and Lee Ross. The theory is concerned with the ways in which people explain (or attribute) the behavior of others or themselves (self attribution) with… …   Wikipedia

  • Harold Kelley — For the Australian rugby league footballer of the same name, see Harold Kelley (rugby league) Harold Kelley (February 16, 1921, Boise, Idaho ndash; January 29, 2003, Malibu, California) was an American social psychologist and professor of… …   Wikipedia

  • Wingless localisation element 3 (WLE3) — The WL3 Wingless localisation element 3 (WLE3) is is an RNA structure that localises the wingless mRNA in flies. The structure consists of a stem, a bulge region, another stem and a loop.The published structure was determined and refined through… …   Wikipedia

  • Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 dimerization initiation site — The Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 dimerization initiation site (HIV 1 DIS) is an RNA structure that is shown to promote dimerization of the HIV 1 genome via a loop loop complex formation.The HIV 1 DIS structure consists of a conserved… …   Wikipedia

  • Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 major splice donor — The Human immunodeficiency virus type 1 major splice donor (HIV 1 SD) is an RNA structure that is shown to modulate splicing efficiency of HIV 1 mRNAs.The HIV 1 SD structure consists of a highly conserved 19 nt stem loop which was predicted… …   Wikipedia

  • Hepatitis B virus PRE alpha — The Hepatitis B virus PREalpha (HBV PREalpha) is an RNA structure that is shown to play a role in nuclear export of HBV mRNAs.HBV PREalpha consists of a 30 nt stem loop, with a 5 nt apical loop. The conserved stem loop was predicted within the… …   Wikipedia

  • Hepatitis B virus PRE beta — The Hepatitis B virus PREbeta (HBV PREbeta) is an RNA structure that is shown to play a role in nuclear export of HBV mRNAs.The minimal HBV PREbeta structure consists of a 23 nt stem loop, with a 9 nt apical loop. The conserved stem loop was… …   Wikipedia

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”